Visual attention

视觉注意
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    虚拟现实中沉浸式体验的一个重要方面是回归,定义为自我运动的错觉。迄今为止,许多文献都探索了通过视觉刺激的操纵来最大化对流的策略(例如,提高速度)或实验背景(例如,研究说明的框架)。然而,个体差异的作用(例如,年龄,生物性别)在对流易感性方面很少受到关注。本研究的目的是调查个体差异因素对年轻人和老年人对流感知的影响。46名年轻成年人(M年龄=25.1)和39名老年人(M年龄=72.4)完成了人格特质的评估,场依赖,在观察旨在诱导圆形对流的运动视觉刺激之前的视觉注意力。使用发作潜伏期的自我报告来测量对流,持续时间,和强度。结果表明,在这两个年龄组中,与男性相比,女性经历了更长的持续时间。此外,场依赖性水平与雄性的对流强度和持续时间有关,而与雌性无关。对流强度的变化最好用生物的混合物来解释,感性的,认知,和个性变量。一起来看,这些发现表明,个体因素对于理解对流易感性的差异很重要.
    An important aspect to an immersive experience in Virtual Reality is vection, defined as the illusion of self-motion. Much of the literature to date has explored strategies to maximize vection through manipulations of the visual stimulus (e.g., increasing speed) or the experimental context (e.g., framing of the study instructions). However, the role of individual differences (e.g., age, biological sex) in vection susceptibility has received little attention. The objective of the current study was to investigate the influence of individual-difference factors on vection perception in younger and older adults. Forty-six younger adults (M age = 25.1) and 39 older adults (M age = 72.4) completed assessments of personality traits, field dependence, and visual attention prior to observing a moving visual stimulus aimed at inducing circular vection. Vection was measured using self-reports of onset latency, duration, and intensity. Results indicated that, in both age groups, females experienced longer-lasting vection compared to males. Additionally, the level of field dependence was related to vection intensity and duration in males but not in females. Variability in vection intensity was best explained by a mixture of biological, perceptual, cognitive, and personality variables. Taken together, these findings suggest that individual factors are important for understanding differences in vection susceptibility.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当环境不强制特定的部署策略时,个人执行目标导向的注意力空间部署的能力与他们更广泛的使用空间注意力的偏好是有区别的。尽管这些偏好可能在我们日常生活中与视觉世界的互动中起着重要作用,他们仍然相对缺乏研究。这项探索性研究调查了关于这些偏好的两个关键问题:首先,在转移注意力与采用注意力广度时,个人的偏好是否一致?其次,还有哪些其他因素与这些偏好相关?在两个实验中,我们测量了参与者偏好如何同时部署注意力广度(使用适应性Navon任务)和眼球运动(使用自由观看任务).我们还测量了参与者的工作记忆能力(实验1),以及他们的个性和世界信仰(实验2)。在这两个实验中,对注意力广度和眼动特征的偏好存在一致的个体差异,但是这两种偏好是相互无关的。工作记忆容量与这些偏好无关。相反,开放体验的人格特质有力地预测了眼动行为偏好的两个方面,因此,较高的开放性水平预测较小的扫视和较短的扫描路径。这表明人格维度可以预测对视觉信息的更多投入的偏好。然而,看来,个人在观看场景时对注意力转移的偏好不一定与他们选择采取的注意力广度有关。
    Individuals\' abilities to perform goal-directed spatial deployments of attention are distinguishable from their broader preferences for how they use spatial attention when circumstances do not compel a specific deployment strategy. Although these preferences are likely to play a major role in how we interact with the visual world during daily life, they remain relatively understudied. This exploratory study investigated two key questions about these preferences: firstly, are individuals consistent in their preferences for how they deploy their spatial attention when making shifts of attention versus adopting an attentional breadth? Secondly, which other factors are associated with these preferences? Across two experiments, we measured how participants preferred to deploy both attentional breadth (using an adapted Navon task) and eye movements (using a free-viewing task). We also measured participants\' working memory capacities (Experiment 1), and their personalities and world beliefs (Experiment 2). In both experiments, there were consistent individual differences in preference for attentional breadth and eye movement characteristics, but these two kinds of preference were unrelated to each other. Working memory capacity was not linked to these preferences. Conversely, the personality trait of Openness to Experience robustly predicted two aspects of eye movement behavior preference, such that higher levels of Openness predicted smaller saccades and shorter scan paths. This suggests that personality dimensions may predict preferences for more absorbed engagement with visual information. However, it appears that individuals\' preferences for shifts of attention during scene viewing do not necessarily relate to the breadth of attention they choose to adopt.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与各种非人类灵长类动物的大量认知研究相比,长臂猿的研究——通常被称为“小猿”——仍然有限,尽管长臂猿对于理解人类和其他类人猿的进化过程很重要。在过去的十年里,已经使用自由参与方法在黑猩猩和其他非人灵长类动物中建立了眼动追踪技术,这不需要受试者的身体约束。我们研究了使用相同方法记录长臂猿视觉扫描路径的可行性。我们试图测量三只长臂猿自发观看图像时的眼球运动,没有事先的固定训练。三个人的校准都很成功,误差小于1度。总的来说,使用了24种刺激,在四分之一的屏幕上展示了景观和非人灵长类动物的面部照片,来测试长臂猿会根据图像类别改变观看时间的预测。所有三只长臂猿观看图像的时间都比背景长,和灵长类动物的面部图像比风景更长。这些结果与先前在其他灵长类动物物种中的发现一致,面部比非面部刺激吸引更多的注意力,表明这种效应在灵长类动物中很常见。这项研究证明了使用长臂猿进行眼动追踪的可行性。对长臂猿视觉探索和认知的进一步研究可能会增强我们对人类智力的系统发育起源以及长臂猿独特进化的理解。
    Compared to the abundance of research on cognition in various nonhuman primate species, studies of gibbons -- often called \"the small apes\" -- remain limited, despite the importance of gibbons for understanding evolutionary processes in humans and other apes. Over the past decade, eye tracking techniques have been established in chimpanzees and other nonhuman primates using the free-participation method, which requires no physical restraint of the subjects. We investigated the feasibility of using the same method to record visual scanpaths in gibbons. We attempted to measure the eye movements of three adult gibbons while they spontaneously viewed images, with no prior fixation training. Calibration was successful in all three individuals, with errors of less than one degree. In total, 24 stimuli were used, with landscape and nonhuman primate face photographs presented on one-quarter of the screen, to test the prediction that gibbons would change their viewing time depending on image category. All three gibbons viewed the images for longer than the background, and primate face images for longer than landscapes. These results are consistent with previous findings in other primate species that faces attract more attention than non-face stimuli, suggesting that this effect is common across primates. This study demonstrates the feasibility of using eye tracking with gibbons. Further studies on gibbon visual exploration and cognition may enhance our understanding of the phylogenetic origins of hominid intelligence as well as the unique evolution of gibbons.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    L-茶氨酸是一种在茶中发现的非蛋白质形成氨基酸。有限的证据表明它可以改善选择性注意力。睡眠不足会损害注意力和精神运动反应,影响汽车驾驶。我们旨在确定在基于交通场景的注意力任务中,L-茶氨酸是否可以改善严重睡眠不足的健康成年人的视觉注意力的神经行为测量。
    在双盲系统中,安慰剂对照,平衡,双向交叉研究,我们比较了200毫克剂量的L-茶氨酸与安慰剂(150毫升蒸馏水)的效果,24名健康志愿者(年龄20-25岁;13名男性)夜间睡眠不足的基于交通场景的视觉识别反应任务。参与者对即将发生的事故现场(即点击)进行了快速按下按钮,而忽略安全的场景。给药前和给药后45分钟进行测试,每次治疗间隔一周。
    在所有会话中命中率均超过90%,在两种治疗中相似,前与后剂量。L-茶氨酸显着减少了错误警报(即对安全场景的响应)(p=0.014),并增加了A\'(即目标干扰物的可辨别性)(p=0.009),而安慰剂没有(p>0.05)。L-茶氨酸将命中反应时间减少38.65ms(p=0.007),和安慰剂19.08ms(p=0.016),然而,从基线的反应时间变化在治疗之间没有显着差异(p>0.05)。
    高剂量的L-茶氨酸似乎可以通过同时提高严重睡眠不足的个体的信息处理速度和目标干扰物的可识别性来改善选择性视觉注意力。这与以前的功能性神经影像学发现一致,其中L-茶氨酸抑制了视觉选择性注意任务中的干扰者处理和默认模式网络活动。
    UNASSIGNED: L-theanine is a non-protein-forming amino acid found in tea. Limited evidence suggests that it improves selective attention. Sleep deprivation impairs attention and psychomotor reactions, affecting automobile driving. We aimed to determine whether L-theanine improves neurobehavioral measures of visual attention in acutely sleep-deprived healthy adults in a traffic-scene-based attention task.
    UNASSIGNED: In a double-blind, placebo-controlled, counterbalanced, two-way crossover study, we compared the effects of a 200-mg dose of L-theanine with a placebo (150 ml of distilled water) on a computerised, traffic-scene-based visual recognition reaction task in 24 healthy volunteers (age 20-25 years; 13 males) sleep-deprived overnight. The participants made speeded button-presses to imminent accident scenes (i.e. hits), while ignoring safe scenes. They were tested pre-dose and 45 min post-dose, each treatment administered one week apart.
    UNASSIGNED: Hit rates were more than 90% in all sessions, and were similar in two treatments, pre- vs post-dose. L-theanine significantly reduced false alarms (i.e. responses to safe scenes) (p = 0.014) and increased A\' (i.e. target-distractor discriminability) (p = 0.009), whereas placebo did not (p > 0.05). L-theanine reduced hit reaction time by 38.65 ms (p = 0.007), and placebo by 19.08 ms (p = 0.016), however reaction time changes from baseline were not significantly different between treatments (p > 0.05).
    UNASSIGNED: L-theanine in high doses appears to improve selective visual attention by concurrently improving information processing speed and target-distractor discriminability in acutely sleep-deprived individuals. This is consistent with previous functional neuroimaging findings, where L-theanine suppressed distractor-processing and default-mode-network activity in visual selective attention tasks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自己孩子的脸是父母最重要的社会刺激之一,和更快的搜索它比其他儿童的脸可能有助于提供更温暖和更敏感的护理。然而,它还没有实验检查父母是否发现他们的孩子的脸更快。此外,虽然自己孩子的脸是经过特殊处理的,搜索自己孩子的脸的时间可能与其他社交刺激的时间相似,如自己或配偶的面孔。这项研究使用视觉搜索范式测试了这些可能性。参与者(父母)搜索他们的孩子,自己,配偶,其他孩子,同性成人,或异性成人的面孔作为搜索目标。我们的研究结果表明,母亲和父亲都比其他孩子更快地识别出孩子的脸。同样,父母比其他成年人更快地找到自己和配偶的面孔。此外,家庭成员\'面孔的搜索时间随着搜索显示中面孔的数量而增加,建议进行注意的连续搜索。这些结果表明,在家庭和亲密关系中学习到的健壮的面部表示可以减少对家庭成员面部的搜索时间。
    Own child\'s face is one of the most socially salient stimuli for parents, and a faster search for it than for other children\'s faces may help provide warmer and more sensitive care. However, it has not been experimentally examined whether parents find their child\'s face faster. In addition, although own child\'s face is specially processed, the search time for own child\'s face may be similar to that for other socially salient stimuli, such as own or spouse\'s faces. This study tested these possibilities using a visual search paradigm. Participants (parents) searched for their child\'s, own, spouse\'s, other child\'s, same-sex adult\'s, or opposite-sex adult\'s faces as search targets. Our findings indicate that both mothers and fathers identified their child\'s face more quickly than other children\'s faces. Similarly, parents found their own and spouse\'s faces more quickly than other adults\' faces. Moreover, the search time for family members\' faces increased with the number of faces on the search display, suggesting an attentional serial search. These results suggest that robust face representations learned within families and close relationships can support reduced search times for family members\' faces.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    近几十年来,人们建立社交联系和获取信息的方式已被技术极大地改变。在线浏览视觉配置文件已成为寻找短期和长期关系的潜在合作伙伴的常用手段。鲜为人知,然而,关于人们在查看在线个人资料时如何优先考虑伴侣质量信息。使用眼动追踪方法和自我报告,这项研究调查了人们如何评估基于个人资料的面部吸引力和基于文本的财务资源信息,以收入和职业为代表。异性恋男性和女性参与者,年龄在18至27岁之间,在使用远程眼动追踪相机记录他们的眼球运动时查看异性资料。根据当前的理论,资源信息对男性对女性面孔的整体关注影响不大,而女性对男性面孔的整体关注取决于收入和职业水平。当收入和职业较低时,女性会更多地评估男性的面孔,不管吸引力。出乎意料的是,然而,男性略微增加了对没有吸引力的女性的关注,这些女性表现出很高的收入和更受人尊敬的职业。男性自我报告对短期关系的女性兴趣更高,而女性自我报告对长期关系的男性兴趣更高。这项工作为进一步研究人们如何浏览基于个人资料的信息和调查伴侣选择过程提供了基础,对在线约会应用程序用户的现实影响,纵断面设计,和内容。
    The way people create social connections and access information has been altered greatly by technology in recent decades. Online browsing of visual profiles has become a common means for seeking potential partners for both short- and long-term relationships. Little is known, however, about how people prioritize mate quality information while viewing online profiles. Using eye-tracking methods and self-report, this study investigated how people evaluated profile-based facial attractiveness and text-based financial resources information, represented by income and occupation. Heterosexual male and female participants, aged between 18 and 27 years, viewed opposite-sex profiles while their eye-movements were recorded using a remote eye-tracking camera. In line with current theory, resources information had little effect on men\'s overall attention to women\'s faces, whereas women\'s overall attention to men\'s faces varied depending on the level of income and occupation. Women evaluated men\'s faces more when income and occupation were low, regardless of attractiveness. Unexpectedly, however, men marginally increased their attention toward unattractive women who showed a high-level of income and more esteemed occupation. Men self-reported a higher interest in women for a short-term relationship and women self-reported a higher interest in men for a long-term relationship. This work provides a foundation to further examine how people browse profile-based information and to investigate the mate selection process, with real-world implications for online dating app users, profile design, and content.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目标:社交媒体上的电子烟营销曝光会影响人们的看法;然而,关于吸引最视觉注意力的营销功能的知识有限。这项研究考察了对一次性电子烟社交媒体营销特征和相关产品认知的视觉关注。
    方法:参与者使用基于计算机的眼动追踪技术查看了来自社交媒体(Instagram)的32张一次性营销帖子图像,以评估营销特征的标准化注意力指标。然后,他们完成了一项评估积极产品认知的调查。这项研究在新泽西进行,美国,2022年6月至9月,包括不使用烟草(n=72)或吸烟(n=42)的年轻人(18-29岁)。
    方法:我们检查了14个营销特征之间的关联(例如产品包装、个人物品,水果/糖果描述符,社交媒体账户)和停留时间(固定持续时间)和进入时间(第一次固定的时间)的标准化注意力指标。然后,我们评估了每个功能的注意力指标与积极产品认知(吸引力和积极使用预期)的关系.
    结果:在所有参与者中,产品描述符的停留时间最长[边际均值(MM)=1.77;95%置信区间(CI)=1.69,1.86],社交媒体账户(MM=1.76;95%CI=1.67,1.85)和水果/糖果描述符特征(MM=1.56;95%CI=1.41,1.70);社交媒体账户的进入时间最短(MM=0.35;95%CI=0.26,0.46),个人项目(MM=0.36;95%CI=0.17,0.56)和人体模型特征(MM=0.40;95%CI=0.08,0.72)。两个使用状态组具有可比的停留时间和进入时间,除了产品描述符功能。产品包装特征的较长停留时间增加了两个使用状态组的积极产品感知[回归系数(β)=0.44和2.61]。水果/糖果描述符(β=1.80)和价格促销功能(β=4.04)的停留时间更长,可以增加吸烟者对产品的正面看法。
    结论:美国年轻人似乎在视觉上特别参与使用社交媒体帐户功能的一次性电子烟营销(帐户个人资料图片,有关上市产品和相关主题标签的信息)和增强产品个人相关性的功能。一次性产品包装,水果/糖果描述符和价格促销可能会增加社交媒体营销在各种使用状态群体中的影响力。
    OBJECTIVE: E-cigarette marketing exposure on social media influences perceptions; however, limited knowledge exists regarding marketing features attracting the most visual attention. This study examined visual attention to features of social media marketing for disposable e-cigarettes and related product perceptions.
    METHODS: Participants viewed 32 disposable marketing post images from social media (Instagram) using computer-based eye-tracking technology to assess standardized attention metrics of marketing features. They then completed a survey assessing positive product perceptions. The study took place in New Jersey, USA, June-September 2022, comprising young adults (aged 18-29) who do not use tobacco (n = 72) or who smoke cigarettes (n = 42).
    METHODS: We examined associations between 14 marketing features (e.g. product package, personal item, fruit/candy descriptor, social media account) and standardized attention metrics of dwell time (fixation duration) and entry time (time to first fixation). Then, we assessed attention metrics for each feature in relation to positive product perceptions (appeal and positive use expectancy).
    RESULTS: Among all participants, dwell time was the longest for the product descriptor [marginal means (MM) = 1.77; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.69, 1.86], social media account (MM = 1.76; 95% CI = 1.67, 1.85) and fruit/candy descriptor features (MM = 1.56; 95% CI = 1.41, 1.70); entry time was the shortest for the social media account (MM = 0.35; 95% CI = 0.26, 0.46), personal item (MM = 0.36; 95% CI = 0.17, 0.56) and human model features (MM = 0.40; 95% CI = 0.08, 0.72). Two use status groups had comparable dwell and entry times, except for the product descriptor feature. Longer dwell time for the product package feature increased positive product perceptions among both use status groups [regression coefficient (β) = 0.44 and 2.61]. Longer dwell time for fruit/candy descriptor (β = 1.80) and price promotion features (β = 4.04) increased positive product perceptions among those who smoke.
    CONCLUSIONS: US young adults appear to be particularly visually engaged by disposable e-cigarette marketing that uses social media account features (account profile pictures, information about the products marketed and relevant hashtags) and features enhancing the products\' personal relatability. Disposable product packages, fruit/candy descriptors and price promotions may increase the influence of social media marketing among various use status groups.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:跌倒在一系列临床队列中很常见,其中常规风险评估通常仅包括主观视觉观察。通常,观察性评估包括在实验室内的脚本步行方案中评估个体的步态,以识别可能增加跌倒风险的缺陷,但是微妙的缺陷可能(不容易)观察到。因此,客观方法(例如,惯性测量单元,IMU)可用于量化高分辨率步态特征,通过捕捉细微的缺陷,实现更明智的跌倒风险评估。然而,仅基于IMU的步态仪器是有限的,未能考虑参与者在环境中的行为和细节(例如,障碍)。基于视频的眼睛跟踪眼镜可以为跌倒风险提供额外的洞察力,阐明人们如何根据头部和眼睛的运动来穿越环境。记录头部和眼睛运动可以提供对视觉注意力对环境刺激的分配如何影响障碍物周围的成功导航的见解。然而,手动审查视频数据以评估头部和眼睛的运动是耗时和主观的。需要一种自动化方法,但目前还不存在。本文提出了一种基于深度学习的物体检测算法(VARFA),用于步行过程中的仪器视觉和视频数据,补充仪表步态。
    方法:该方法自动标记在步态实验室中捕获的视频数据,以评估视觉注意力和环境细节。所提出的算法使用YoloV8模型进行训练,并使用基于实验室的新数据集。
    结果:VARFA取得了出色的评估指标(0.93mAP50),识别,和定位静态对象(例如,步行路径中的障碍物),平均准确率为93%。同样,基于U-NET的航迹/路径分割模型取得了良好的指标(IoU0.82),表明预测的轨迹(即,步行路径)与实际轨道紧密对齐,有82%的重叠。值得注意的是,这两个模型都实现了这些指标,同时以实时速度处理,展示务实应用的效率和有效性。
    结论:仪器化方法通过评估视觉上的注意力分配(即,导航过程中关于某人何时何地出席的信息),提高这一领域仪器的广度。将VARFA用于仪器视觉可用于通过提供行为和背景数据来补充仪器来更好地告知跌倒风险评估,例如,步态任务期间的IMU数据。这可能值得注意(例如,个性化)康复影响广泛的临床队列,其中步态差和跌倒风险增加是常见的。
    BACKGROUND: Falls are common in a range of clinical cohorts, where routine risk assessment often comprises subjective visual observation only. Typically, observational assessment involves evaluation of an individual\'s gait during scripted walking protocols within a lab to identify deficits that potentially increase fall risk, but subtle deficits may not be (readily) observable. Therefore, objective approaches (e.g., inertial measurement units, IMUs) are useful for quantifying high resolution gait characteristics, enabling more informed fall risk assessment by capturing subtle deficits. However, IMU-based gait instrumentation alone is limited, failing to consider participant behaviour and details within the environment (e.g., obstacles). Video-based eye-tracking glasses may provide additional insight to fall risk, clarifying how people traverse environments based on head and eye movements. Recording head and eye movements can provide insights into how the allocation of visual attention to environmental stimuli influences successful navigation around obstacles. Yet, manual review of video data to evaluate head and eye movements is time-consuming and subjective. An automated approach is needed but none currently exists. This paper proposes a deep learning-based object detection algorithm (VARFA) to instrument vision and video data during walks, complementing instrumented gait.
    METHODS: The approach automatically labels video data captured in a gait lab to assess visual attention and details of the environment. The proposed algorithm uses a YoloV8 model trained on with a novel lab-based dataset.
    RESULTS: VARFA achieved excellent evaluation metrics (0.93 mAP50), identifying, and localizing static objects (e.g., obstacles in the walking path) with an average accuracy of 93%. Similarly, a U-NET based track/path segmentation model achieved good metrics (IoU 0.82), suggesting that the predicted tracks (i.e., walking paths) align closely with the actual track, with an overlap of 82%. Notably, both models achieved these metrics while processing at real-time speeds, demonstrating efficiency and effectiveness for pragmatic applications.
    CONCLUSIONS: The instrumented approach improves the efficiency and accuracy of fall risk assessment by evaluating the visual allocation of attention (i.e., information about when and where a person is attending) during navigation, improving the breadth of instrumentation in this area. Use of VARFA to instrument vision could be used to better inform fall risk assessment by providing behaviour and context data to complement instrumented e.g., IMU data during gait tasks. That may have notable (e.g., personalized) rehabilitation implications across a wide range of clinical cohorts where poor gait and increased fall risk are common.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    照顾者与婴儿的互动塑造了婴儿的早期视觉体验;然而,低收入和中等收入国家(LMIC)在表征这些互动的视觉认知动态方面的工作有限。这里,我们提出了一种创新的二进视觉认知管道,使用机器学习方法捕获,进程,并分析了跨文化护理者与婴儿互动的视觉动态。我们进行了两项研究,以检查其在低(印度农村)和高(英国城市)资源环境中的应用。研究1开发并验证了处理使用头戴式摄像机和眼睛跟踪器捕获的护理人员-婴儿互动数据的管道。我们使用面部检测和对象识别网络,并使用12个照顾者-婴儿二元组(来自6个月大的英国队列的4二元组,来自一个6个月大的印度队列的4个二分体,和来自一个9个月大的印度队列的4个双组)。结果表明,人脸和玩具检测具有鲁棒性和准确性,以及处理后的和手动编码的二进交互之间的高百分比一致性。研究2将管道应用于更大的数据集(来自英国的25个6个月大的孩子,来自印度的31名6个月大的孩子,和来自印度的37个9个月大的孩子),目的是比较两种文化背景下护理人员与婴儿互动的视觉动态。结果表明,跨文化视觉探索的关键措施之间存在显著的对应关系,包括在婴儿主导的联合注意力发作期间较长的平均外观持续时间。此外,我们发现了不同文化的一些差异。最值得注意的是,与西方中产阶级家庭中常见的以儿童为中心的育儿观点一致,英国婴儿导致的联合注意力发作比例较高.总之,我们报告的管道提供了一个客观的评估工具,可以量化高资源和低资源环境中照顾者-婴儿互动的视觉动态.
    Caregiver-infant interactions shape infants\' early visual experience; however, there is limited work from low-and middle-income countries (LMIC) in characterizing the visual cognitive dynamics of these interactions. Here, we present an innovative dyadic visual cognition pipeline using machine learning methods which captures, processes, and analyses the visual dynamics of caregiver-infant interactions across cultures. We undertake two studies to examine its application in both low (rural India) and high (urban UK) resource settings. Study 1 develops and validates the pipeline to process caregiver-infant interaction data captured using head-mounted cameras and eye-trackers. We use face detection and object recognition networks and validate these tools using 12 caregiver-infant dyads (4 dyads from a 6-month-old UK cohort, 4 dyads from a 6-month-old India cohort, and 4 dyads from a 9-month-old India cohort). Results show robust and accurate face and toy detection, as well as a high percent agreement between processed and manually coded dyadic interactions. Study 2 applied the pipeline to a larger data set (25 6-month-olds from the UK, 31 6-month-olds from India, and 37 9-month-olds from India) with the aim of comparing the visual dynamics of caregiver-infant interaction across the two cultural settings. Results show remarkable correspondence between key measures of visual exploration across cultures, including longer mean look durations during infant-led joint attention episodes. In addition, we found several differences across cultures. Most notably, infants in the UK had a higher proportion of infant-led joint attention episodes consistent with a child-centered view of parenting common in western middle-class families. In summary, the pipeline we report provides an objective assessment tool to quantify the visual dynamics of caregiver-infant interaction across high- and low-resource settings.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    视觉系统有节奏地运行,通过及时协调的感知和注意过程,涉及alpha范围(7-13Hz)中的共存模式,在~10Hz,和θ(3-6Hz)范围,分别。在这里,我们旨在消除任务需求中的变化是否存在歧义,在注意力需求和目标呈现方面,可能会影响行为视觉表现中感知或注意成分的出现,还揭示了两个大脑半球的采样机制可能存在的差异。为了这个目标,视觉空间性能在两个版本的视觉检测任务中被密集采样,其中目标呈现的一侧是固定的(任务1),参与者监测一个单一的半球,或者在试验中随机变化,参与者同时监测两个半球(任务2)。通过光谱分解对性能进行了分析,揭示行为振荡模式。对于任务1,当注意力资源只集中在一个半球时,结果显示了一个振荡模式,波动幅度在10赫兹和6-9赫兹,对于左侧和右侧偏场的刺激,分别,可能代表在左半球和右半球内具有不同效率的感知采样机制。对于任务2,当注意力资源同时部署到两个半场时,对于向左和向右呈现的刺激,都出现了5赫兹的节奏,反映了一个注意的抽样过程,同样由两个半球支撑。总的来说,结果表明,不同的感知和注意采样机制在不同的振荡频率下起作用,其患病率和半球偏侧化取决于任务要求。
    The visual system operates rhythmically, through timely coordinated perceptual and attentional processes, involving coexisting patterns in the alpha range (7-13 Hz) at ∼10 Hz, and theta (3-6 Hz) range, respectively. Here we aimed to disambiguate whether variations in task requirements, in terms of attentional demand and side of target presentation, might influence the occurrence of either perceptual or attentional components in behavioral visual performance, also uncovering possible differences in the sampling mechanisms of the two cerebral hemispheres. To this aim, visuospatial performance was densely sampled in two versions of a visual detection task where the side of target presentation was fixed (Task 1), with participants monitoring one single hemifield, or randomly varying across trials, with participants monitoring both hemifields simultaneously (Task 2). Performance was analyzed through spectral decomposition, to reveal behavioral oscillatory patterns. For Task 1, when attentional resources where focused on one hemifield only, the results revealed an oscillatory pattern fluctuating at ∼10 Hz and ∼6-9 Hz, for stimuli presented to the left and the right hemifield, respectively, possibly representing a perceptual sampling mechanism with different efficiency within the left and the right hemispheres. For Task 2, when attentional resources were simultaneously deployed to the two hemifields, a ∼5 Hz rhythm emerged both for stimuli presented to the left and the right, reflecting an attentional sampling process, equally supported by the two hemispheres. Overall, the results suggest that distinct perceptual and attentional sampling mechanisms operate at different oscillatory frequencies and their prevalence and hemispheric lateralization depends on task requirements.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号