Symbolism

象征主义
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    符号系统(SS)是生命系统的独特产物,这样,象征主义和生活可能是密不可分的交织现象。在给定的SS内,存在在其上对信令进行功能优化的符号复杂度的范围。此范围相对于潜在的复杂且可能无限大的背景而存在,未使用的符号空间。了解符号集如何对这个潜在空间进行采样与包括生物化学和语言学在内的各个领域有关。我们定量探索了两个生物符号学系统的图形复杂性:遗传编码氨基酸(GEAA)和书面语言。复杂的分子和图形概念与GEAA和书面语言高度相关。符号集相对于它们的潜在空间通常既不是最小的也不是最大的复杂,但存在于一个客观可定义的分布中,GEAAs具有特别低的复杂性。引导这些不同系统的选择压力可以通过符号产生和歧义消除效率来解释。这些选择压力可能是普遍的,提供一个可量化的指标进行比较,并建议宇宙中的所有生命都可以发现相对于其潜在空间的最佳符号集复杂性分布。如果是,SS的单个组件的“复杂性”可能不如符号集复杂性分布那么强。
    Symbolic systems (SSs) are uniquely products of living systems, such that symbolism and life may be inextricably intertwined phenomena. Within a given SS, there is a range of symbol complexity over which signaling is functionally optimized. This range exists relative to a complex and potentially infinitely large background of latent, unused symbol space. Understanding how symbol sets sample this latent space is relevant to diverse fields including biochemistry and linguistics. We quantitatively explored the graphic complexity of two biosemiotic systems: genetically encoded amino acids (GEAAs) and written language. Molecular and graphical notions of complexity are highly correlated for GEAAs and written language. Symbol sets are generally neither minimally nor maximally complex relative to their latent spaces, but exist across an objectively definable distribution, with the GEAAs having especially low complexity. The selection pressures guiding these disparate systems are explicable by symbol production and disambiguation efficiency. These selection pressures may be universal, offer a quantifiable metric for comparison, and suggest that all life in the Universe may discover optimal symbol set complexity distributions with respect to their latent spaces. If so, the \"complexity\" of individual components of SSs may not be as strong a biomarker as symbol set complexity distribution.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项实验研究调查了婴儿是否在语音和手势提示中使用象似性来解释单词含义。具体来说,我们测试了婴儿对大小的声音象征和标志性手势提示的敏感性,并询问以多模式方式组合这些提示是否会以超加性方式增强婴儿的敏感性。三十六个14-17个月大的婴儿参加了一项偏爱的任务,在该任务中,他们听到了说出的非单词(例如,\“zudzud\”)同时观察一个小的和大的物体(例如,一个小而大的正方形)。所有婴儿都被呈现为对象大小的标志性提示(小或大)(1)在口语非单词的音高(高与低),(2)在手势(小或大),或(3)音高和手势一致(例如,表示小正方形的高音调和小手势)。在任何标志性提示条件下,婴儿都不喜欢相同大小的物体。贝叶斯因子分析显示了对零假设的中等到强烈的支持。总之,14-17个月大的婴儿没有使用标志性的音调提示,标志性的手势提示,或标志性的多模态提示(音高和手势)将语音与它们的对象相关联。这些发现挑战了强调象似性在早期语言发展中的作用的理论。
    This experimental study investigated whether infants use iconicity in speech and gesture cues to interpret word meanings. Specifically, we tested infants\' sensitivity to size sound symbolism and iconic gesture cues and asked whether combining these cues in a multimodal fashion would enhance infants\' sensitivity in a superadditive manner. Thirty-six 14-17-month-old infants participated in a preferential looking task in which they heard a spoken nonword (e.g., \"zudzud\") while observing a small and large object (e.g., a small and large square). All infants were presented with an iconic cue for object size (small or large) (1) in the pitch of the spoken non-word (high vs low), (2) in gesture (small or large), or (3) congruently in pitch and gesture (e.g., a high pitch and small gesture indicating a small square). Infants did not show a preference for congruently sized objects in any iconic cue condition. Bayes factor analyses showed moderate to strong support for the null hypotheses. In conclusion, 14-17-month-old infants did not use iconic pitch cues, iconic gesture cues, or iconic multimodal cues (pitch and gesture) to associate speech sounds with their referents. These findings challenge theories that emphasize the role of iconicity in early language development.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    电磁波和光频率范围内的低可检测性和伪装技能为自然生物提供了生存优势,对于理解生物圈的运行原理至关重要。从自然共生共生中获得灵感,本文提出了一种基于超色散表面的共生电磁阴影伪装机制,旨在研究其对特定物体可观察性的影响。设计和实验结果表明,共生阴影二面角可以显着降低整体散射量,与对比对象相比,在12-18GHz频率范围内至少收缩10dB。与已知的伪装方法不同,电磁阴影技术在没有任何涂层和屏蔽金属目标的情况下缩小整体散射,同时可能为隐蔽物体提供广泛的功能设计自由度,生物,或设备。这也为探索自然界中与共生相关的伪装现象提供了线索。
    Low detectability and camouflage skills in the electromagnetic wave and light frequency range provide survival advantages for natural creatures and are essential for understanding the operational principles of the biosphere. Taking inspiration from natural mutualistic symbiosis, this paper proposes a symbiotic electromagnetic shadow camouflage mechanism based on a superdispersive surface, aiming to investigate its impact on the observability of specific objects. The design and experimental results indicate that the symbiotic shadow dihedral can significantly reduce overall scattering quantity, which reaches at least 10 dB shrink in the 12-18 GHz frequency range compared to the contrast object. Unlike known camouflage methods, the electromagnetic shadow technology shrinks the overall scattering without any coating and shield metal target while probably offering extensive functional design freedom for the concealed object, creature, or equipment. This also provides a hint to explore symbiosis-related camouflage phenomena in nature.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究的目的是通过弗洛伊德和荣格精神分析的概念棱镜来分析哈萨克斯坦的当代后现代文学作品。为了实现研究目标,使用了以下方法:公理,内容分析,和比较。研究结果确定,当代哈萨克族作家通过文本揭示了大量的动机和思想,符号,和字符。弗洛伊德和荣格的心理学方法是解释他们的有力工具,这是精神分析的标准,有自己的语义内容的特定特征。对后现代材料的内容分析已经确定,哈萨克故事追溯了神话的动机,宗教,关系和内在的精神发展,其中考虑了故事情节中英雄的心理差异。在作品的精神分析过程中,有人强调,哈萨克斯坦文学中的后现代主义反映了对绝对真理的拒绝,模糊流派之间的界限,玩传统的形式和内容。故事中的许多人物都经历着身份危机,通过弗洛伊德三合会和荣格的原型图像进行了分析。哈萨克文学,融入国家的文化和历史遗产,反映了心态的特征,社会文化转型,英雄的身份和精神追求。
    The aim of the study is to analyse contemporary postmodern literary works of Kazakhstan through the conceptual prism of Freudian and Jungian psychoanalysis. To achieve research goals, the following methods were used: axiomatic, content analysis, and comparative. The results of the study determined that contemporary Kazakh writers characterise a large field of motives and ideas that are revealed through text, symbols, and characters. Strong tools for their interpretation were the psychological approaches of Freud and Jung, which are the standards of psychoanalysis and have their own specific features of semantic content. Content analysis of postmodern materials has established that Kazakh stories trace the motives of mythology, religion, relationships and inner spiritual development, which consider the mental differences of the heroes of the storylines. During the psychoanalysis of the works, it was emphasised that postmodernism in the literature of Kazakhstan reflects the rejection of absolute truths, blurring the boundaries between genres, playing with traditional forms and content. Many of the characters in the stories are experiencing an identity crisis, which has been analysed through the Freudian triad and Jung\'s archetypal images. Kazakh literature, being woven into the cultural and historical heritage of the nation, reflects the features of mentality, socio-cultural transformations, identity and spiritual quest of heroes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一个语音可以通过象似性与多个含义相关联,索引性,和/或系统性。直到最近,声音象征主义的“多潜能”才引起了人们的广泛关注,它仍然没有调查多潜能可能如何产生。在目前的研究中,日本人,韩语,普通话,说英语的人在三个语义尺度上对不熟悉的珠宝名称进行评级:大小,亮度,和硬度。结果显示了特定语言和跨语言共享的多潜在声音象征意义。日本人将声音与大而暗的珠宝联系在一起,而说普通话的人则将[我]与小而明亮的珠宝联系在一起。日本人,普通话,说英语的人也将嘴唇圆润与黑暗和柔软联系在一起。这些声音象征意义不太可能通过隐喻或转喻的扩展来获得,也没有报道他们会进行colexify。值得注意的是,在没有嘴唇舍入中介的纯语义网络中,柔软度可以与亮度相关联,如Yawaraka-nahizashi/jawaakanaçizai/“一个温柔的(点燃的。软)阳光“在日语中。这些发现表明,声音象征主义的语义网络可能与隐喻或转喻的语义网络不一致。当前的研究以(phono)语义图的形式总结了这些发现,以促进跨语言比较多潜在的声音象征主义。
    One speech sound can be associated with multiple meanings through iconicity, indexicality, and/or systematicity. It was not until recently that this \"pluripotentiality\" of sound symbolism attracted serious attention, and it remains uninvestigated how pluripotentiality may arise. In the current study, Japanese, Korean, Mandarin, and English speakers rated unfamiliar jewel names on three semantic scales: size, brightness, and hardness. The results showed language-specific and cross-linguistically shared pluripotential sound symbolism. Japanese speakers associated voiced stops with large and dark jewels, whereas Mandarin speakers associated [i] with small and bright jewels. Japanese, Mandarin, and English speakers also associated lip rounding with darkness and softness. These sound-symbolic meanings are unlikely to be obtained through metaphorical or metonymical extension, nor are they reported to colexify. Notably, in a purely semantic network without the mediation of lip rounding, softness can instead be associated with brightness, as illustrated by synesthetic metaphors such as yawaraka-na hizashi /jawaɾakanaçizaɕi/ \"a gentle (lit. soft) sunshine\" in Japanese. These findings suggest that the semantic networks of sound symbolism may not coincide with those of metaphor or metonymy. The current study summarizes the findings in the form of (phono)semantic maps to facilitate cross-linguistic comparisons of pluripotential sound symbolism.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    化学象征主义的炼金术概念,命名法,亲和力在1770年代和1820年代之间发生了根本性的变化,与化学革命大致同时(约1772-89),用拉瓦锡的新化学代替发素理论。使用旧的,炼金术符号,伯格曼设计了一个公式系统来描述几乎所有已知的无机化学,他影响了GuitondeMorveau的Mémoire,以及随后的Méthode命名法。Hassenfratz和Adet设计了一种新的人工手语,然而,过于复杂和不直观,无法获得广泛的接受。伯格曼完善了亲和力的概念,但他对燃眉之急的信念迅速使该系统过时。Wenzel意识到金属在酸中的溶解不仅是亲和力的问题,而且是浓度的问题。他和Berthollet分别制定了《大众行动法》的早期版本,从而试图量化亲和力。里希特提出了一个被称为等效比例定律的原则,描述酸碱反应和盐的双重分解,而是继续使用伯格曼式的炼金术公式。只有约翰·道尔顿的原子理论用小球来表示原子及其结合成分子,用炼金术符号进行了明确的突破。
    The alchemical concepts of chemical symbolism, nomenclature, and affinity underwent fundamental changes between the 1770s and the 1820s, roughly simultaneously with the Chemical Revolution (ca. 1772-89), i. e. the replacement of the phlogiston theory with Lavoisier\'s New Chemistry. Using the old, alchemical symbols, Bergman devised a system of formulas to describe virtually all known inorganic chemistry, and he influenced Guiton de Morveau\'s Mémoire sur les Dénominations Chimiques, and the subsequent Méthode de nomenclature. Hassenfratz and Adet devised a new artificial sign language which, however, was too complicated and unintuitive to gain widespread acceptance. Bergman refined the concept of affinity, but his belief in phlogiston rapidly made the system obsolete. Wenzel realized that the dissolution of metals in acids is not just a question of affinity but rather of concentration, and he and Berthollet separately formulated early versions of the Law of Mass Action thereby making attempts to quantify affinity redundant. Richter formulated a principle that became known as the Law of Equivalent Proportions, describing acid-base reactions and double decompositions of salts, but continuing to use Bergman-style alchemical formulas. Only John Dalton\'s atomic theory with little globules denoting atoms and their combination into molecules made a definite break with the alchemical symbols.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:自杀是导致死亡的主要原因,将自杀预防作为主要的公共卫生重点。增加对影响自杀行为的因素的理解至关重要。先前的研究暗示了心理上的亲密关系,以感知一个人对特定物体的感觉有多近/远或附着/分离为特征,作为影响自杀行为的认知因素。然而,需要更好地理解相关人群对自杀方法的心理贴近性是如何概念化的,以改进其评估并了解其如何赋予自杀风险.
    方法:目标是通过纳入相关人群的反馈来完善心理接近自杀方法的概念化。我们对那些主要被认为有自杀经历的人进行了30次采访(n=10),与自杀患者一起工作的临床医生(n=11),和自杀研究人员(n=9)。使用NVivo软件的严格主题方法用于发现常见主题。
    结果:主要主题包括熟悉度,comfort,和依恋,一个新兴的象征主义主题。我们定义这些主题,为它们的含义提供上下文,并在不同的参与者中分享示范报价。
    结论:我们考虑临床,研究,从跨学科的角度和政策含义,并讨论本研究的优势和局限性。
    BACKGROUND: Suicide is a leading cause of death, making suicide prevention a major public health priority. Increasing understanding of factors influencing suicidal behavior is paramount. Previous research has implicated psychological closeness, characterized by perceptions of how close/distant or attached/detached one feels to a particular object, as a cognitive factor that influences suicidal behavior. However, a better understanding of how psychological closeness to suicide methods is conceptualized by relevant populations is needed to improve its assessment and understand how it may confer risk for suicide.
    METHODS: The goal was to refine the conceptualization of psychological closeness to suicide methods by incorporating feedback from relevant populations. We conducted 30 interviews with those primarily identifying as having lived experience of suicide (n = 10), clinicians who work with suicidal patients (n = 11), and suicide researchers (n = 9). A rigorous thematic approach using NVivo software was used to uncover common themes.
    RESULTS: Primary themes included familiarity, comfort, and attachment, with one emerging theme of symbolism. We define these themes, provide context to their meaning, and share exemplary quotes across diverse participants.
    CONCLUSIONS: We consider clinical, research, and policy implications from an interdisciplinary lens and discuss the strengths and limitations of this study.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    已经表明,读取元音[i]和辅音[t]有助于精确的抓握响应,而[α]和[k]与更快的功率抓地力响应相关。当参与者用小的或大的响应键执行响应时,已经观察到类似的效果。本研究调查了当大声朗读语音单元(实验1)或无声朗读语音单元(实验2)时,元音和辅音是否会通过抓握反应和按键产生不同的效果。作为第二个目标,这项研究调查了最近观察到的效果,其中视觉刺激的上部位置与高元音的较快发声相关联,下部位置与低元音相关联,可以在手动响应链接中观察到,例如,具有上键响应的[i]和具有较低响应的[α]。首先,研究表明,当辅音被公开表达时,只有握力响应才能观察到交互效应,虽然元音的产生被证明会系统地影响小/大按键,以及精度/功率抓地力响应。其次,元音[i]和辅音[t]与上部反应相关,而[α]和[k]与较低的反应相关,特别是在公开的衔接任务中。本文深入研究了这些语音元素的潜在声音符号含义,表明它们的声学和发音特性可能会隐含地将它们与特定的响应幅度对齐,垂直位置,和握把类型。
    It has been shown that reading the vowel [i] and consonant [t] facilitates precision grip responses, while [ɑ] and [k] are associated with faster power grip responses. A similar effect has been observed when participants perform responses with small or large response keys. The present study investigated whether the vowels and consonants could produce different effects with the grip responses and keypresses when the speech units are read aloud (Experiment 1) or silently (Experiment 2). As a second objective, the study investigated whether the recently observed effect, in which the upper position of a visual stimulus is associated with faster vocalizations of the high vowel and the lower position is associated with the low vowel, can be observed in manual responses linking, for example, the [i] with responses of the upper key and [ɑ] with lower responses. Firstly, the study showed that when the consonants are overtly articulated, the interaction effect can be observed only with the grip responses, while the vowel production was shown to systematically influence small/large keypresses, as well as precision/power grip responses. Secondly, the vowel [i] and consonant [t] were associated with the upper responses, while [ɑ] and [k] were associated with the lower responses, particularly in the overt articulation task. The paper delves into the potential sound-symbolic implications of these phonetic elements, suggesting that their acoustic and articulatory characteristics might implicitly align them with specific response magnitudes, vertical positions, and grip types.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究使用机器学习算法调查了日语名称中性别的声音符号表达。本研究的主要目的是探讨性别是如何在构成日语名字的音素中表达的,以及系统的声音意义映射是否,在印欧语中观察到,延伸到日本人。除此之外,这项研究比较了机器学习算法的性能。随机森林和XGBoost算法使用名称的声音和参照物的典型性别作为因变量进行训练。使用k-fold交叉验证(28倍)对每个算法进行交叉验证,并在训练周期中未包括的样品上进行测试。两种算法都被证明在将姓名分类为性别类别时相当准确;然而,XGBoost模型的性能明显优于随机森林算法。特征重要性评分显示某些声音携带性别信息。即,浊音双唇鼻/m/和无音velar辅音/k/与女性气质有关,高前元音/i/与阳刚之气有关。/i/和/k/所观察到的关联与其他语言中的典型模式相反,这表明日本人在性别的声音象征性表达方面是独一无二的。这项研究强调了在声音象征主义研究中考虑文化和语言细微差别的重要性,并强调了XGBoost在捕获数据中复杂关系以提高分类准确性方面的优势。这些发现有助于理解语言中的声音象征意义和性别关联。
    This study investigates the sound symbolic expressions of gender in Japanese names with machine learning algorithms. The main goal of this study is to explore how gender is expressed in the phonemes that make up Japanese names and whether systematic sound-meaning mappings, observed in Indo-European languages, extend to Japanese. In addition to this, this study compares the performance of machine learning algorithms. Random Forest and XGBoost algorithms are trained using the sounds of names and the typical gender of the referents as the dependent variable. Each algorithm is cross-validated using k-fold cross-validation (28 folds) and tested on samples not included in the training cycle. Both algorithms are shown to be reasonably accurate at classifying names into gender categories; however, the XGBoost model performs significantly better than the Random Forest algorithm. Feature importance scores reveal that certain sounds carry gender information. Namely, the voiced bilabial nasal /m/ and voiceless velar consonant /k/ were associated with femininity, and the high front vowel /i/ were associated with masculinity. The association observed for /i/ and /k/ stand contrary to typical patterns found in other languages, suggesting that Japanese is unique in the sound symbolic expression of gender. This study highlights the importance of considering cultural and linguistic nuances in sound symbolism research and underscores the advantage of XGBoost in capturing complex relationships within the data for improved classification accuracy. These findings contribute to the understanding of sound symbolism and gender associations in language.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号