Social perception

社会感知
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的社会道德发展理论认为,人类进化出一种在不同类型的互动中评估他人社会行为的能力。先前的婴儿研究发现,亲社会的触角和视觉偏好都超过了反社会药物。然而,是否可以通过触达行为和视觉注意力部署来推断积极或消极效价对参与积极追逐互动的代理人行为的归因(即,视觉注意力的脱离)仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。在这里,我们介绍了7个月大的婴儿(N=92),这些事件显示出积极的追逐互动。通过使用优先到达和注意任务(即,重叠范式),我们评估了婴儿是否以及如何评估攻击性追逐互动.结果表明,年幼的婴儿更喜欢接触受害者而不是侵略者,但这两种药剂都不会影响视觉注意力。此外,只有当动态线索和情感面部特征与代理人的社会角色一致时,这种达到的偏好才会出现。总的来说,这些发现表明,婴儿对攻击性互动的评估是基于婴儿对某些运动学线索的敏感性,这些线索表征了代理的作用,尤其是,这些动作和代理人的面部情感表达之间的一致性。
    Recent theories of socio-moral development assume that humans evolved a capacity to evaluate others\' social actions in different kinds of interactions. Prior infant studies found both reaching and visual preferences for the prosocial over the antisocial agents. However, whether the attribution of either positive or negative valence to agents\' actions involved in an aggressive chasing interaction can be inferred by both reaching behaviors and visual attention deployment (i.e., disengagement of visual attention) is still an open question. Here we presented 7-month-old infants (N = 92) with events displaying an aggressive chasing interaction. By using preferential reaching and an attentional task (i.e., overlap paradigm), we assessed whether and how infants evaluate aggressive chasing interactions. The results demonstrated that young infants prefer to reach the victim over the aggressor, but neither agent affects visual attention. Moreover, such reaching preferences emerged only when dynamic cues and emotional face-like features were congruent with agents\' social roles. Overall, these findings suggested that infants\' evaluations of aggressive interactions are based on infants\' sensitivity to some kinematic cues that characterized agents\' actions and, especially, to the congruency between such motions and the face-like emotional expressions of the agents.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们感兴趣的是,处于浪漫关系中的人们会如何看待他们的伴侣的智力,他们有高或低的特质愤怒。具体来说,我们提到了在择偶时同情(低愤怒)和能力(高智商)之间的紧张关系。一些进化理论表明,交配可能被认为是这两个高阶属性之间的讨价还价过程。我们的研究涉及148对恋爱关系中的异性恋夫妇。我们测量了关系满意度之间的关系,特质愤怒,客观智力,自我评估的智力,并主观评估合作伙伴的智力。我们发现愤怒的男人在他们的浪漫关系中比那些不那么愤怒的男人更不满意,他们的伴侣对这段关系也不太满意。此外,女人认为愤怒的男人不那么聪明,在控制了男人的客观智力后仍然存在的效果。最后,我们发现,女性对伴侣智力的感知在男性的愤怒和男女关系满意度之间起了中介作用。我们的研究结果表明,愤怒和智力在浪漫关系的运作中起着重要作用,与强调能力价值的进化理论(即,智力)和同情心(即,低愤怒)在浪漫的伴侣中。此外,我们的研究强调了女性对伴侣智力的感知在决定关系质量方面的重要性。
    We were interested in how people in a romantic relationship would perceive the intelligence of their partners who have high or low trait anger. Specifically, we referred to the tension between compassion (low anger) and competence (high intelligence) in mate choice. Some evolutionary theories suggest that mating might be considered a bargaining process between these two higher-order attributes. Our study involved 148 heterosexual couples in romantic relationships. We measured the relationship between relationship satisfaction, trait anger, objective intelligence, self-assessed intelligence, and subjectively assessed partners\' intelligence. We found that angrier men were less satisfied in their romantic relationship than those men who were less angry, and their partners were also less satisfied in the relationship. Additionally, women perceived angrier men as less intelligent, an effect that remained after controlling for men\'s objective intelligence. Lastly, we found that women\'s perception of their partner\'s intelligence mediated the link between men\'s anger and relationship satisfaction for both sexes. Our findings suggest that both anger and intelligence play important roles in romantic relationship functioning, consistent with evolutionary theories that emphasize the value of competence (i.e., intelligence) and compassion (i.e., low anger) in romantic partners. Furthermore, our study highlights the importance of women\'s perception of their partner\'s intelligence in determining the quality of the relationship.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多专门为临床人群开发的社会认知评估现在正在本科生人群中广泛使用,要么提供临床组的比较,要么探索从健康到亚临床人群的连续体的表现。然而,在一般人群中使用这些评估的适当性尚不清楚.目前的研究,因此,试图确定在本科生样本中使用时,社会认知心理测量评估(SCOPE)电池是否保留其心理测量特性。最初的SCOPE电池用于265名本科生参与者,通过评估地板和天花板效应来衡量任务的效用。内部一致性,报告每个任务的判别有效性和收敛有效性,并评估了任务绩效与神经认知和社会功能之间的关系。其中一些任务显示出有限的实用性,Hinting任务特别展示了明显的天花板效应。只有两种衡量社会认知偏见的方法达到了令人满意的内部一致性。选择的任务证明小,但与社会功能结果显著相关,并显著预测2-3%的方差高于单独的神经认知。尽管与社会功能有关,结果表明,在临床样本的初始SCOPE研究中证明的心理测量特性并未完全扩展到健康的本科生人群,并且突出了对社会认知任务的需求,该任务可以有效地评估整个连续体的能力.
    Many social cognitive assessments that were developed specifically for use in clinical populations are now being widely used in undergraduate populations, either to provide a comparison for clinical groups or to explore performance across the continuum from healthy to subclinical populations. However, the appropriateness of using these assessments in the general population is unclear. The current study, therefore, seeks to determine whether the Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation (SCOPE) battery retains its psychometric properties when used in an undergraduate sample.The initial SCOPE battery was administered to 265 undergraduate participants, and the utility of the tasks was measured by assessing floor and ceiling effects. Internal consistency, discriminant and convergent validity are reported for each task, and the relationship between task performance and neurocognition and social functioning were assessed.Several of the tasks demonstrated limited utility, with the Hinting Task specifically demonstrating a pronounced ceiling effect. Only two measures of social cognitive biases achieved satisfactory internal consistency. Select tasks demonstrated small, but significant relationships with social functioning outcomes and significantly predicted 2-3% of variance above neurocognition alone.Despite the association with social functioning, results indicate that the psychometric properties demonstrated in the initial SCOPE study with a clinical sample do not fully extend to a healthy undergraduate population and highlight the need for social cognitive tasks that validly assess ability across the continuum.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    情感感知与我们的思考和说话方式相互作用,包括我们的情感概念。身体表达是情感交流的重要方式,但它的感知是否以及如何被概念知识调节是未知的。在这项研究中,我们采用代表性相似性分析,并结合语义相似性进行了三个实验,鼠标跟踪任务,以及脑电图和功能磁共振成像技术的一次行为任务,结果表明,概念知识预测了身体表情的感性表征。Further,这种预测效应发生在刺激后约170ms.情感概念知识影响了梭状回和舌回中身体表情的神经编码。一起来看,我们的结果表明,情感类别的概念知识塑造了腹侧视觉皮层中身体表情的结构表示,这为构建的情绪理论提供了令人信服的证据。
    Emotion perception interacts with how we think and speak, including our concept of emotions. Body expression is an important way of emotion communication, but it is unknown whether and how its perception is modulated by conceptual knowledge. In this study, we employed representational similarity analysis and conducted three experiments combining semantic similarity, mouse-tracking task, and one-back behavioral task with electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging techniques, the results of which show that conceptual knowledge predicted the perceptual representation of body expressions. Further, this prediction effect occurred at approximately 170 ms post-stimulus. The neural encoding of body expressions in the fusiform gyrus and lingual gyrus was impacted by emotion concept knowledge. Taken together, our results indicate that conceptual knowledge of emotion categories shapes the configural representation of body expressions in the ventral visual cortex, which offers compelling evidence for the constructed emotion theory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的研究表明,我们的大脑可能会产生微妙的,虚假的运动信号从其他人流向他们关注的对象,辅助社会认知。例如,短暂暴露于描绘其他人凝视着物体的静态图像,使得受试者在检测到随后的注视方向上的运动时变慢,这表明看着别人的凝视会导致定向运动适应。在这里我们确认,使用更严格的方法,观察另一个人注视特定方向的静态图像,在一个物体上,产生了相反方向的运动后遗症。后效表现为检测左与左的感知决策阈值的变化。右运动。当这个人远离物体时,效果消失了。这些发现表明,他人的专心注视被编码为隐含的代理到对象的运动,该运动足够健壮以引起真正的运动后效,虽然微妙到足以保持亚阈值。
    Recent work suggests that our brains may generate subtle, false motion signals streaming from other people to the objects of their attention, aiding social cognition. For instance, brief exposure to static images depicting other people gazing at objects made subjects slower at detecting subsequent motion in the direction of gaze, suggesting that looking at someone else\'s gaze caused a directional motion adaptation. Here we confirm, using a more stringent method, that viewing static images of another person gazing in a particular direction, at an object, produced motion aftereffects in the opposite direction. The aftereffect was manifested as a change in perceptual decision threshold for detecting left vs. right motion. The effect disappeared when the person was looking away from the object. These findings suggest that the attentive gaze of others is encoded as an implied agent-to-object motion that is sufficiently robust to cause genuine motion aftereffects, though subtle enough to remain subthreshold.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在患有创伤性脑损伤(TBI)的个体中,面部刺激传递的社会信息的处理发生了变化。这项研究调查了这些改变是否也会影响视觉注意力定向对眼睛注视信号的反应机制。TBI患者和符合相关标准的健康个体对照组完成了空间提示任务。在这项任务中,横向视觉目标与任务无关的面孔一起呈现,目光向左或向右转移。指向左或右的箭头也被用作非社会控制刺激。通过基于面部刺激传达的情绪表达和精神状态的解码测试,进一步研究了社会认知能力。TBI组的情绪和精神状态解码比对照组差。然而,两组都表现出对眼睛注视和箭头刺激的可靠且可比的注意力定向。尽管TBI患者在社交面部处理的某些方面存在缺陷,在这种神经心理学人群中,注意力的凝视提示似乎得以保留。
    The processing of social information transmitted by facial stimuli is altered in individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI). This study investigated whether these alterations also affect the mechanisms underlying the orienting of visual attention in response to eye-gaze signals. TBI patients and a control group of healthy individuals matched on relevant criteria completed a spatial cueing task. In this task, a lateral visual target was presented along with a task-irrelevant face, with the gaze averted to the left or right. Arrows pointing towards the left or right were also used as non-social control stimuli. Social cognition abilities were further investigated through tests based on decoding emotional expressions and mental states conveyed by facial stimuli. The decoding of emotions and mental states was worse in the TBI group than in the control group. However, both groups demonstrated reliable and comparable orienting of attention to both eye-gaze and arrow stimuli. Despite impairments in certain aspects of social face processing among TBI patients, gaze cueing of attention appears to be preserved in this neuropsychological population.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:自闭症谱系障碍(ASD),一种神经发育障碍,由社会交往缺陷加上重复的行为和有限的兴趣定义,目前影响普通人群中的1/36儿童。脑功能成像的最新进展显示有望提供ASD诊断可能性的有用生物标志物,行为特征严重性,甚至对治疗干预的反应。然而,当前的金标准神经成像方法(例如,功能磁共振成像,由于受约束的成像环境,fMRI)在对ASD相关行为的基础脑功能的自然研究中受到限制。与功能磁共振成像相比,高密度扩散光学层析成像(HD-DOT),一种非侵入性和最小约束的光学神经成像模式,可以克服这些限制。在这里,我们旨在建立HD-DOT,以评估自闭症和非自闭症学龄儿童的脑功能,因为他们执行了先前显示的生物运动知觉任务,可产生与ASD诊断和行为特征相关的结果.
    方法:我们使用HD-DOT对46名ASD学龄期参与者和49名非自闭症个体(NAI)的大脑功能进行成像,因为他们观察了相干生物学和乱序运动的动态点光显示。我们通过统计参数映射评估了组水平的皮质脑功能。此外,我们测试了大脑行为与自闭症特征维度指标的关联,用社会反应能力量表-2,用分层回归模型衡量。
    结果:我们发现,NAI参与者在与视觉相关的皮质区域中表现出比ASD儿童更强的大脑活动对比(相干>乱序),电机,社会处理。此外,回归模型揭示了自闭症参与者的多个皮质区域,这些区域的脑功能与ASD特征的维度测量显著相关.
    结论:光学成像方法的深度敏感性有限,因此无法测量皮质下深部区域的大脑活动。然而,该HD-DOT系统的视野包括先前涉及自闭症的基于任务和无任务研究的多个大脑区域。
    结论:这项研究表明,HD-DOT对脑功能敏感,既区分NAI和ASD组,又与ASD特征的维度测量相关。这些发现将HD-DOT确立为研究自闭症和非自闭症儿童大脑功能的有效工具。此外,这项研究建立了与生物运动知觉相关的神经相关性及其与ASD特征的维度测量的关联。
    Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder defined by social communication deficits plus repetitive behaviors and restricted interests, currently affects 1/36 children in the general population. Recent advances in functional brain imaging show promise to provide useful biomarkers of ASD diagnostic likelihood, behavioral trait severity, and even response to therapeutic intervention. However, current gold-standard neuroimaging methods (e.g., functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI) are limited in naturalistic studies of brain function underlying ASD-associated behaviors due to the constrained imaging environment. Compared to fMRI, high-density diffuse optical tomography (HD-DOT), a non-invasive and minimally constraining optical neuroimaging modality, can overcome these limitations. Herein, we aimed to establish HD-DOT to evaluate brain function in autistic and non-autistic school-age children as they performed a biological motion perception task previously shown to yield results related to both ASD diagnosis and behavioral traits.
    We used HD-DOT to image brain function in 46 ASD school-age participants and 49 non-autistic individuals (NAI) as they viewed dynamic point-light displays of coherent biological and scrambled motion. We assessed group-level cortical brain function with statistical parametric mapping. Additionally, we tested for brain-behavior associations with dimensional metrics of autism traits, as measured with the Social Responsiveness Scale-2, with hierarchical regression models.
    We found that NAI participants presented stronger brain activity contrast (coherent > scrambled) than ASD children in cortical regions related to visual, motor, and social processing. Additionally, regression models revealed multiple cortical regions in autistic participants where brain function is significantly associated with dimensional measures of ASD traits.
    Optical imaging methods are limited in depth sensitivity and so cannot measure brain activity within deep subcortical regions. However, the field of view of this HD-DOT system includes multiple brain regions previously implicated in both task-based and task-free studies on autism.
    This study demonstrates that HD-DOT is sensitive to brain function that both differentiates between NAI and ASD groups and correlates with dimensional measures of ASD traits. These findings establish HD-DOT as an effective tool for investigating brain function in autistic and non-autistic children. Moreover, this study established neural correlates related to biological motion perception and its association with dimensional measures of ASD traits.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Systematic Review
    面部情绪感知缺陷,疾病进展和转化诊断表型的可能指标,通过对35项研究(2567名CHR个体,1103非过渡[CHR-NT],212过渡[CHR-T],512首发精神病[FEP],和1936年健康对照[HC])。CHR显示总体(g=-0.369[95%CI,-0.485至-0.253])和检测愤怒的特定障碍,厌恶,恐惧,幸福,中立,与HC相比,悲伤,除了惊喜。FEP显示比CHR普遍不足(g=-0.378[95%CI,-0.509至-0.247]),和CHR-T显示比CHR-NT更明显的基线损伤(g=-0.217[95%CI,-0.365至-0.068])。FEP只表现出较差的感知恐惧的能力,但不是其他个体的情绪,与CHR相比。无论过渡状态如何(CHR-NT和CHR-T),在感知个体情绪方面都观察到类似的表现。然而,文献比较了FEP中个体情绪的感知,CHR-T,CHR是有限的。这项研究主要描述了CHR中面部情绪感知的一般和整体损伤,可以预测过渡风险,强调需要对情绪感知的多模态参数以及与其他精神病结果的关联进行未来研究。
    Facial emotion perception deficits, a possible indicator of illness progression and transdiagnostic phenotype, were examined in high-risk psychosis (CHR) patients through a systematic review and meta-analysis of 35 studies (2567 CHR individuals, 1103 non-transitioned [CHR-NT], 212 transitioned [CHR-T], 512 first-episode psychosis [FEP], and 1936 healthy controls [HC]). CHR showed overall (g = -0.369 [95 % CI, -0.485 to -0.253]) and specific impairments in detecting anger, disgust, fear, happiness, neutrality, and sadness compared to HC, except for surprise. FEP revealed a general deficit than CHR (g = -0.378 [95 % CI, -0.509 to -0.247]), and CHR-T displayed more pronounced baseline impairments than CHR-NT (g = -0.217 [95 % CI, -0.365 to -0.068]). FEP only exhibited a poorer ability to perceive fear, but not other individual emotions, compared to CHR. Similar performances in perceiving individual emotions were observed regardless of transition status (CHR-NT and CHR-T). However, literature comparing the perception of individual emotions among FEP, CHR-T, and CHR is limited. This study primarily characterized the general and overall impairments of facial emotion perception in CHR which could predict transition risk, emphasizing the need for future research on multimodal parameters of emotion perception and associations with other psychiatric outcomes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    婴儿的非语言表达——灿烂的微笑或尖锐的哭声——能引起强烈的反应。尽管父母对自己的婴儿的反应表达相对较好理解,在这里,我们研究了成年人是否对不熟悉的婴儿表现出更普遍的行为和生理反应,从而产生各种表情。我们在为人父母之前招募了美国新兴成年人(N=84),18-25岁,68%的女性,种族(20%的西班牙裔/拉丁美洲人)和种族(7%的亚洲人,13%黑色,中东1%70%白色8%多种族)多样化。他们观察了四个80年代不熟悉的2到6个月大的哭泣的音频视频剪辑,微笑,打哈欠,平静地坐着(情绪中立的控制)。每个编辑视频描绘了9个不同的婴儿(总共36个剪辑)。我们发现成年人在行为和生理上都有反映:婴儿微笑时的面部表情更加积极,以及更负面的面部表情和瞳孔扩张-表明婴儿哭闹的觉醒增加。观察婴儿打哈欠时,成年人也打哈欠更多,瞳孔扩张更多。一起,这些发现表明,即使是非父母的新兴成年人对不熟悉的婴儿的表情也高度敏感,他们自然地“抓住”(即,行为和生理镜子),即使没有指示。在人类进化史的过程中,这种敏感性可能被选择用于,促进成人“处理前语言婴儿”的表达,以满足他们的需求。
    Infants\' nonverbal expressions-a broad smile or a sharp cry-are powerful at eliciting reactions. Although parents\' reactions to their own infants\' expressions are relatively well understood, here we studied whether adults more generally exhibit behavioral and physiological reactions to unfamiliar infants producing various expressions. We recruited U.S. emerging adults (N = 84) prior to parenthood, 18-25 years old, 68% women, ethnically (20% Hispanic/Latino) and racially (7% Asian, 13% Black, 1% Middle Eastern, 70% White, 8% multiracial) diverse. They observed four 80-s audio-video clips of unfamiliar 2- to 6-month-olds crying, smiling, yawning, and sitting calmly (emotionally neutral control). Each compilation video depicted 9 different infants (36 clips total). We found adults mirrored behaviorally and physiologically: more positive facial expressions to infants smiling, and more negative facial expressions and pupil dilation-indicating increases in arousal-to infants crying. Adults also yawned more and had more pupil dilation when observing infants yawning. Together, these findings suggest that even nonparent emerging adults are highly sensitive to unfamiliar infants\' expressions, which they naturally \"catch\" (i.e., behaviorally and physiologically mirror), even without instructions. Such sensitivity may have-over the course of humans\' evolutionary history-been selected for, to facilitate adults\' processing of preverbal infants\' expressions to meet their needs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    反刻板印象挑战了性别类型的信念对人们的职业愿望和生活方式选择产生的有害影响。令人惊讶的是,然而,如何容易地获得意想不到的人相关知识的关键问题仍然知之甚少。因此,在两个实验中,改变目标的面部外观以操纵刻板印象拟合优度(即,高vs.低女性气质/男性气质),在这里,我们使用了一个概率选择任务来探索反刻板印象和刻板印象个体的学习率。是否职业(Expt.1)或与性状相关(Expt。2)探讨了性别刻板印象,计算分析产生了一致的结果。强调令人惊讶的信息的效力(即,面部不适),与预期的人相比,意外的知识获取加速,在反刻板和刻板的学习环境中。这些发现肯定了社会感知的预测性描述,并说明了旨在减少实验室外刻板印象的干预措施的最佳特征。
    Counterstereotypes challenge the deleterious effects that gender-typed beliefs exert on people\'s occupational aspirations and lifestyle choices. Surprisingly, however, the critical issue of how readily unexpected person-related knowledge can be acquired remains poorly understood. Accordingly, in two experiments in which the facial appearance of targets was varied to manipulate goodness-of-stereotype-fit (i.e., high vs. low femininity/masculinity), here we used a probabilistic selection task to probe the rate at which counter-stereotypic and stereotypic individuals can be learned. Whether occupational (Expt. 1) or trait-related (Expt. 2) gender stereotypes were explored, a computational analysis yielded consistent results. Underscoring the potency of surprising information (i.e., facial misfits), knowledge acquisition was accelerated for unexpected compared to expected persons, both in counter-stereotypic and stereotypic learning contexts. These findings affirm predictive accounts of social perception and speak to the optimal characteristics of interventions designed to reduce stereotyping outside the laboratory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号