Simon effect

西蒙效应
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    适应行为是认知控制和执行功能的基础。这项研究调查了认知控制机制和情节特征检索如何相互作用以影响适应性,特别关注theta(4至8Hz)振荡动力学。我们进行了两种不同的西蒙任务,包含不兼容的响应,响应兼容,中立的审判。实验1证明了认知调整-特异性,不兼容试验后的认知屏蔽和兼容试验后的认知放松-反映在与西蒙效应相关的中额叶theta功率调制中。实验2表明,减少试验之间的特征重叠导致行为和中额叶theta活动的顺序调节不太明显,支持认知控制和特征整合共享共同神经机制的假设。这些发现强调了认知控制过程和情节特征整合在调节行为中的相互作用。结果倡导将自上而下和自下而上的过程结合起来的混合模型作为一个全面的框架来理解认知控制动力学和自适应行为。
    Adaptive behavior is fundamental to cognitive control and executive functioning. This study investigates how cognitive control mechanisms and episodic feature retrieval interact to influence adaptiveness, focusing particularly on theta (4 to 8 Hz) oscillatory dynamics. We conducted two variations of the Simon task, incorporating response-incompatible, response-compatible, and neutral trials. Experiment 1 demonstrated that cognitive adjustments-specifically, cognitive shielding following incompatible trials and cognitive relaxation following compatible ones-are reflected in midfrontal theta power modulations associated with the Simon effect. Experiment 2 showed that reducing feature overlap between trials leads to less pronounced sequential modulations in behavior and midfrontal theta activity, supporting the hypothesis that cognitive control and feature integration share a common neural mechanism. These findings highlight the interaction of cognitive control processes and episodic feature integration in modulating behavior. The results advocate for hybrid models that combine top-down and bottom-up processes as a comprehensive framework to understand cognitive control dynamics and adaptive behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在许多任务中,人类可以用速度来换取准确性。策略的这种变化对不同冲突任务中的一致性效果具有不同的后果。最近,Mittelstädt等人。(2022)表明,这些差异与通过三角洲图评估的一致性效应的动态有关。随着Eriksen侧翼任务一致性效果在精度集下的增量图增加,随着西蒙任务中三角洲地块的减少,它们变小了。在这里,我们测试了单个任务的假设,利用以下观察结果:当西蒙任务增量图首先呈现不相关特征时,但当相关功能导致时增加。速度和准确性指令下的一致性效应之间的差异证实了与delta图斜率的假设关系。事实上,对于在比较的速度-精度条件下的类似delta图,这种关系应该是在速度和精度设定的情况下反应时间越来越短的直接结果,分别。然而,当同时呈现相关和不相关的特征时,在所有反应时间设定的速度下,一致性效应更强。对于这种情况,具有扩展泄漏的基于模型的补充分析,竞争累加器模型表明不相关刺激特征的影响更强、更持久。当δ图减小时,反应时间的一致性效应伴随着错误率的一致性效应,但不是在增加的时候。
    In many tasks humans can trade speed against accuracy. This variation of strategy has different consequences for congruency effects in different conflict tasks. Recently, Mittelstädt et al. (2022) suggested that these differences are related to the dynamics of congruency effects as assessed by delta plots. With increasing delta plots in the Eriksen flanker task congruency effects were larger under accuracy set, and with decreasing delta plots in the Simon task they were smaller. Here we tested the hypothesis for a single task, making use of the observation that for the Simon task delta plots decline when the irrelevant feature is presented first, but increase when the relevant feature leads. The differences between congruency effects under speed and accuracy instructions confirmed the hypothesized relation to the slope of delta plots. In fact, for similar delta plots in the compared speed-accuracy conditions, the relation should be a straightforward consequence of the shorter and longer reaction times with speed and accuracy set, respectively. However, when relevant and irrelevant features were presented simultaneously, congruency effects were stronger under speed set at all reaction times. For this condition, a supplementary model-based analysis with an extended leaky, competing accumulator model suggested a stronger and longer-lasting influence of the irrelevant stimulus feature. The congruency effects for reaction times were accompanied by congruency effects for error rates when delta plots were decreasing, but not when they were increasing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语言中介(LB)是一种非正式的翻译经验,双语者充当家庭成员的语言和文化中介。LB可能有长期的社会情感和认知结果,然而,人们对其对执行功能(EF)的影响知之甚少。这项研究考察了与语言经纪经验相关的第一语言(L1)熟练程度和负面情绪如何影响西蒙任务(ST)的EF表现。
    53名具有LB经验的墨西哥裔美国人西班牙语-英语双语者进行了ST,并报告了他们对母亲LB的感受。
    使用线性混合效应模型分析了正确ST试验的平均反应时间(RT)和准确率,试用类型,与LB经验相关的熟练程度和负面情绪作为因素,它们的相互作用作为额外的预测因素。
    与经纪经历相关的L1熟练程度和负面情绪存在分歧,但对EF的综合影响。与我们的假设相反,低L1熟练度预示着更好的表现,对于低L1熟练度和低负面情绪经纪经历的经纪人,西蒙效应最小。然而,无论与经纪经历相关的负面情绪如何,较高的L1熟练程度都可以预测更好的表现(最小的RTs)。
    本研究从不同的角度考察双语者的个体差异,在其中,我们研究了与经纪经历以及L1熟练程度相关的负面情绪如何与EF表现相关。
    我们的结果为了解双语经验中的个体差异的需要提供了支持,例如L1熟练程度和与LB相关的负面情绪,与ST上的表现互动。
    UNASSIGNED: Language brokering (LB) is an informal translation experience where bilinguals serve as linguistic and cultural intermediaries for family members. LB may have long-term socio-emotional and cognitive outcomes, yet little is known about its effects on executive functions (EFs). This study examines how first language (L1) proficiency and negative emotions tied to language brokering experiences affect EF performance on a Simon task (ST).
    UNASSIGNED: Fifty-three Mexican American Spanish-English bilinguals with LB experience performed a ST, and reported their feelings towards LB for their mother.
    UNASSIGNED: Mean reaction times (RTs) and accuracy rates for correct ST trials were analyzed using linear mixed effects modeling, with trial type, proficiency and negative emotions tied to LB experience as factors and their interactions as additional predictors.
    UNASSIGNED: The L1 proficiency and negative emotions tied to brokering experiences have divergent, but combined effects on EF. Contrary to our hypotheses, low L1 proficiency predicted better performance and the smallest Simon effect was found for brokers with low L1 proficiency and low negative emotional brokering experiences. However, high L1 proficiency predicted better performance (smallest RTs) regardless of negative emotions tied to brokering experiences.
    UNASSIGNED: This study takes a different perspective on the examination of individual differences among bilinguals, in which we examine how negative emotions tied to brokering experiences coupled with L1 proficiency relates to EF performance.
    UNASSIGNED: Our results provide support for the need to understand how individual differences in bilingual language experiences, such as L1 proficiency and negative emotions tied to LB, interact with performance on the ST.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与环境的有效互动需要使自己的行为适应环境需求。我们这样做是通过使用来自我们环境的线索,并将这些线索与我们的身体联系起来来预测事件的结果。最近关于内化认知的文献表明,与任务相关的刺激,靠近手,接收更多的注意力能力,并与刺激不同的处理方式,在空间上离我们的身体更远。还提出了近手处理有利于冲突解决。在目前的研究中,通过将提示范式(视觉注意力的分配)与近手空间中的冲突处理范式(西蒙任务)相结合,我们在先前工作的背景下测试了对近手空间的注意力偏向的假设。此外,处理的相关性是通过使用情感(愤怒与中性微笑)凝视线索来操纵的(即,改变线索的效价)。我们的结果表明(a)效价×提示一致性×手接近度的相互作用是显着的,表明在近端条件下,负价的提示效应更大。(B)效价×西蒙相容性×刺激-手邻近相互作用的相互作用显著,表明对于负价处理,近端的Simon效应小于远端刺激手的情况.在中性价条件下,这种作用至少在数值上是有效的,但没有明显逆转。(c)总体而言,提示一致性,指示对目标刺激开始的正确和不正确的注意力分配,没有发现对西蒙相容性×刺激手接近度有任何影响。我们的结果表明,效价,注意力的分配,和冲突,似乎是决定手邻近效应的方向和强度的决定性因素。
    An effective interaction with the environment requires adaptation of one\'s own behaviour to environmental demands. We do so by using cues from our environment and relating these cues to our body to predict the outcomes of events. The recent literature on embodied cognition suggests that task-relevant stimuli, presented near the hands, receive more attentional capacity and are processed differently than stimuli, presented spatially more distant to our body. It has also been proposed that near-hand processing is beneficial to conflict resolution. In the current study, we tested the assumption of an attentional bias towards the near hand space in the context of our previous work by combining a cueing paradigm (allocation of visual attention) with a conflict processing paradigm (Simon task) in the near vs far hand space. In addition, the relevance of processing was manipulated by using affective (angry vs neutral smileys) gaze cues (i.e., varying the valence of the cues). Our results indicate that (a) the interaction of valence × cue congruency × hand proximity was significant, indicating that the cueing effect was larger for negative valence in the proximal condition. (b) The interaction of valence × Simon compatibility × stimulus-hand proximity interaction was significant, indicating that for negative valence processing, the Simon effect was smaller in the proximal than in the distal stimulus-hand condition. This effect was at least numerically but not significantly reversed in the neutral valence condition. (c) Overall, cue congruency, indicating the correct vs incorrect attention allocation to the target stimulus onset, did not reveal any effect on Simon compatibility × stimulus-hand proximity. Our results suggest that valence, the allocation of attention, and conflict, seem to be decisive factors determining the direction and strength of hand proximity effects.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    返回抑制(IOR)是指对先前提示位置处呈现的目标的较慢响应。在各种眼睛运动条件下的目标辨别性能的对比表明,反射动眼系统的激活水平决定了效果的性质。值得注意的是,当主动抑制反射动眼神经系统时,观察到更接近处理连续体输入端的提示的抑制作用,并且当反射动眼系统被主动参与时,观察到更接近处理连续体的输出端的抑制作用。此外,这两种形式的IOR与西蒙效应相互作用不同。漂移扩散建模表明,从理论上讲,两个参数可以解释基于输出形式的IOR所带来的速度-精度折衷:阈值增加和试验噪声减少。在实验1中,我们证明了阈值参数通过使用混合的区分和定位目标来测量IOR,从而最好地解释了基于输出的IOR形式。实验2采用响应信号方法,表明基于输出的形式对有关目标身份的信息的积累没有影响。这些结果与响应偏差解释了IOR的输出形式。
    Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to slower responses to targets presented at previously cued locations. Contrasting target discrimination performance over various eye movement conditions has shown the level of activation of the reflexive oculomotor system determines the nature of the effect. Notably, an inhibitory effect of a cue nearer to the input end of the processing continuum is observed when the reflexive oculomotor system is actively suppressed, and an inhibitory effect nearer the output end of the processing continuum is observed when the reflexive oculomotor system is actively engaged. Furthermore, these two forms of IOR interact differently with the Simon effect. Drift diffusion modeling has suggested that two parameters can theoretically account for the speed-accuracy tradeoff rendered by the output-based form of IOR: increased threshold and decreased trial noise. In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that the threshold parameter best accounts for the output-based form of IOR by measuring it with intermixed discrimination and localization targets. Experiment 2 employed the response-signal methodology and showed that the output-based form has no effect on the accrual of information about the target\'s identity. These results converge with the response bias account for the output form of IOR.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当参与者通过用各自的食指按左或右键来响应与任务相关的刺激属性时,如果与任务无关的左右刺激位置对应于响应键的位置,则反应时间比不对应的时间短。对于右撇子来说,对于右侧的刺激,这种西蒙效应比左侧的刺激更大;对于左撇子,这种西蒙效应的不对称性是相反的。对于右脚用脚按压踏板,也发现了类似的不对称性。对于将刺激因素和反应位置因素分开的分析,这些不对称性表现为响应位置的主要影响,在主要效应器的情况下反应更快。如果西蒙效应不对称性严格地是效应子优势的函数,左脚用脚回应时,它应该反转。在实验1中,左占优势的人表现出左手比右手更快的反应,而右手比左脚更快,这一发现与先前关于轻击动作的研究一致。右占主导地位的人也表现出右脚不对称,但是,出乎意料的是,不是典型的手反应不对称。为了评估手压产生的结果是否与手指压不同,在实验2中,参与者通过手指按压和手按压来完成Simon任务.在两种响应模式下,左右主导人的相对不对称性都很明显。我们的结果与西蒙效应不对称性主要是由于效应子效率的差异的观点一致,通常但不总是偏爱显性效应物。
    When participants respond to a task-relevant stimulus attribute by pressing a left or right key with the respective index finger, reaction time is shorter if task-irrelevant left-right stimulus location corresponds to that of the response key than if it does not. For right-handers, this Simon effect is larger for right-located than left-located stimuli; for left-handers this Simon-effect asymmetry is reversed. A similar asymmetry has been found for right-footers pressing pedals with their feet. For analyses that separate stimulus- and response-location factors, these asymmetries appear as a main effect of response location, with responses being faster with the dominant effector. If the Simon-effect asymmetry is strictly a function of effector dominance, it should reverse for left-footers responding with their feet. In Experiment 1, left-dominant persons showed faster responses with the left than right hand but with the right than left foot, a finding consistent with prior research on tapping actions. Right-dominant persons also showed the right-foot asymmetry but, unexpectedly, not the typical asymmetry with hand responses. To evaluate whether hand-presses yield results distinct from finger-presses, in Experiment 2 participants performed the Simon task with finger-presses and hand-presses. The opposing asymmetries for right- and left-dominant persons were evident for both response modes. Our results are consistent with the view that the Simon effect asymmetry is primarily due to differences in effector efficiency, usually but not always favoring the dominant effector.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类行为对各种兼容性现象都有怀疑。例如,对刺激出现的一侧的反应比对另一侧的反应更快,称为刺激-反应(S-R)兼容性。这甚至是真的,如果反应是针对不同的刺激特征,而位置本身是无关紧要的(西蒙兼容性)。此外,反应通常会对环境产生可感知的影响。如果他们以可预测的方式这样做,如果它们产生一个(例如,空间)同一侧与另一侧的兼容效果。它,如果左反应可预测地产生左效应比产生右效应更快。这种效应被称为响应效应(R-E)相容性。最后,刺激和效果之间也可能存在兼容性,这相应地称为刺激-效应(S-E)相容性。这种兼容性现象也与应用目的有关,无论是腹腔镜手术还是航空。本研究调查了无接触手势交互的Simon和R-E兼容性。根据最近的一项研究,没有观察到R-E相容性的影响,然而,无关紧要的刺激位置产生了巨大的西蒙效应。因此,与通过(其他)工具(例如杠杆)进行的交互相比,无触摸手势在兼容性现象方面的表现似乎有所不同。
    Human actions are suspect to various compatibility phenomena. For example, responding is faster to the side where a stimulus appears than to the opposite side, referred to as stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility. This is even true, if the response is given to a different stimulus feature, while location itself is irrelevant (Simon compatibility). In addition, responses typically produce perceivable effects on the environment. If they do so in a predictable way, responses are faster if they produce a (e.g., spatially) compatible effect on the same side than on the other side. That it, a left response is produced faster if it results predictably in a left effect than in a right effect. This effect is called response-effect (R-E) compatibility. Finally, compatibility could also exist between stimuli and the effects, which is accordingly called stimulus-effect (S-E) compatibility. Such compatibility phenomena are also relevant for applied purposes, be it in laparoscopic surgery or aviation. The present study investigates Simon and R-E compatibility for touchless gesture interactions. In line with a recent study, no effect of R-E compatibility was observed, yet irrelevant stimulus location yielded a large Simon effect. Touchless gestures thus seem to behave differently with regard to compatibility phenomena than interactions via (other) tools such as levers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以前的研究表明,在冲突下做出感知决定时,不正确的运动激活,但是运动过程在解决冲突中的潜在参与仍不清楚。本研究测试了当预期的运动处理需求增加时,是否可以减少分心信息的影响。具体来说,在两个追踪老鼠的西蒙实验中,我们操纵了块状电机需求(高与低)通过要求参与者将鼠标光标移动到屏幕上显示的大与小(实验1)或近与远(实验2)响应框。我们认为,参与者会在运动需求高和低的情况下增加动作控制,这将减少基于位置的激活的干扰作用。结果支持这一假设:在高电机需求和低电机需求下,西蒙效应降低,即使通过分布分析控制基于干扰物的激活中的时变波动,这种调制也保持不变(即,三角洲地块)。因此,目前的研究结果表明,对不同运动成本的预期会影响冲突处理。我们建议,基于干扰因素的激活和基于目标的激活之间的竞争在预期运动需求的运动前和/或运动阶段有偏差,还讨论了动作控制的替代实现。
    Previous studies have shown incorrect motor activation when making perceptual decisions under conflict, but the potential involvement of motor processes in conflict resolution is still unclear. The present study tested whether the effects of distracting information may be reduced when anticipated motor processing demands increase. Specifically, across two mouse-tracking Simon experiments, we manipulated blockwise motor demands (high vs. low) by requiring participants to move a mouse cursor to either large versus small (Experiment 1) or near versus far (Experiment 2) response boxes presented on the screen. We reasoned that participants would increase action control in blocks with high versus low motor demands and that this would reduce the distracting effect of location-based activation. The results support this hypothesis: Simon effects were reduced under high versus low motor demands and this modulation held even when controlling for time-varying fluctuations in distractor-based activation via distributional analyses (i.e., delta plots). Thus, the present findings indicate that anticipation of different motor costs can influence conflict processing. We propose that the competition between distractor-based and target-based activation is biased at premotor and/or motor stages in anticipation of motor demands, but also discuss alternative implementations of action control.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的研究表明,绩效偶然性奖励可以通过偏置西蒙任务中不相关的位置-反应关联来调节认知控制。然而,在无关的单词(Stroop任务)或无关的侧翼(Eriksen侧翼任务)的情况下,奖励的影响尚不清楚。在两个预先注册的实验中,本研究调查了不同类型干扰物的奖励对冲突处理的影响。在西蒙任务(实验1和2)中,当不一致与一致的试验得到奖励时,冲突对平均反应时间(RT)的影响减少了,并且在Eriksen侧翼任务(实验2)中也观察到奖励对冲突处理的调节作用,但不是在Stroop任务中(实验1)。我们建议,对干扰者特定的奖励突发事件的认知控制调整可以在干扰者类型中推广,从而产生与感知相关的(侧翼任务)和与运动相关的(西蒙任务)冲突,但是,如果有的话,当干扰者产生额外的更高级别的任务冲突(Stroop任务)时,在有限的程度上。此外,分布RT分析(delta图)表明,奖励的干扰者反应关联不仅通过偏置强度(Simon和Eriksen任务)而且还通过抑制干扰者处理(Eriksen任务)的时间过程来调节认知控制。总的来说,本研究分离了奖励对认知控制的干扰因素-一般和干扰因素-特异性影响。
    Previous studies have suggested that performance-contingent reward can modulate cognitive control by biasing irrelevant location-response associations in the Simon task. However, the influence of reward in the case of irrelevant words (Stroop task) or irrelevant flankers (Eriksen Flanker task) remains unclear. Across two preregistered experiments, the present study investigated the influence of reward on conflict processing with different types of distractors. Conflict effects on mean reaction time (RT) were reduced in the Simon task (Experiments 1 and 2) when incongruent versus congruent trials were rewarded, and this modulating effect of reward on conflict processing was also observed in the Eriksen flanker task (Experiment 2), but not in the Stroop task (Experiment 1). We propose that cognitive control adjustments to distractor-specific reward contingencies can be generalized across distractor types producing both perceptual-related (Flanker task) and motor-related (Simon task) conflict, but, if any, to a limited degree when distractors produce additional higher-level task conflict (Stroop task). In addition, distributional RT analyses (delta plots) revealed that rewarded distractor-response associations modulate cognitive control not only via biasing the strength (Simon and Eriksen tasks) but also the time-course of suppressing distractor processing (Eriksen task). Overall, the present study dissociated distractor-general and distractor-specific effects of reward on cognitive control.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的研究表明,文化会影响感知和注意力。这些研究通常涉及西方人与东亚人的比较,受文化中自我概念或个人主义-集体主义光谱上的立场的假设差异的激励。然而,文化差异的其他潜在重要来源被忽视了,例如由城市空间环境形成的交通指向性差异(即,左手vs.右侧交通)。因此,现有的研究可能会在解释认知中观察到的文化差异时过分强调自我概念或个人主义-集体主义维度。本研究使用西蒙任务调查了空间认知,并测试了来自四个国家(澳大利亚,中国,德国,和马来西亚)在文化取向(集体主义与个人主义)和交通指向性(左手与右侧交通)。该任务使用了Simon效应的两个可能的参考框架:一个基于相对于屏幕中心的全局刺激位置的以身体为中心的框架,另一个基于相对于上下文对象的局部刺激位置的以对象为中心的框架。不出所料,对于两个空间参考框架,所有组都显示出可靠的西蒙效应。然而,全球西蒙效应在来自左手交通国家的参与者中更大。相比之下,当地的西蒙效应是由文化取向的差异调节的,对集体主义文化的参与者产生了更大的影响。这种模式表明两种文化差异的来源,viz.文化取向和交通指向性,以不同的方式促进空间认知的差异。
    Previous research has suggested that culture influences perception and attention. These studies have typically involved comparisons of Westerners with East Asians, motivated by assumed differences in the cultures\' self-concept or position on the individualism-collectivism spectrum. However, other potentially important sources of cultural variance have been neglected, such as differences in traffic directionality shaped by the urban spatial environment (i.e., left-hand vs. right-hand traffic). Thus, existing research may potentially place too much emphasis on self-concepts or the individualism-collectivism dimension in explaining observed cultural differences in cognition. The present study investigated spatial cognition using a Simon task and tested participants from four nations (Australia, China, Germany, and Malaysia) that differ in both cultural orientation (collectivistic vs. individualistic) and traffic directionality (left-hand vs. right-hand traffic). The task used two possible reference frames underlying the Simon effect: a body-centered one based on global stimulus position relative to the screen\'s center versus an object-centered one based on local stimulus position relative to a context object. As expected, all groups showed a reliable Simon effect for both spatial reference frames. However, the global Simon effect was larger in participants from countries with left-hand traffic. In contrast, the local Simon effect was modulated by differences in cultural orientation, with larger effects in participants from collectivistic cultures. This pattern suggests that both sources of cultural variation, viz. cultural orientation and traffic directionality, contribute to differences in spatial cognition in distinct ways.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号