Self-focus

自我聚焦
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    新兴成年期是以自我为中心的青年轨迹,身份探索,青春期和成年之间的感觉,不稳定性,和实验。这一轨迹最初是在具有性别平等和技术进步的工业化个人主义国家中确定的。为了衡量向成年的过渡,创建了新兴成年期维度清单(IDEA)。虽然成年被认为是普遍的,12个国家对问卷的调整显示出不同的模式,它的跨文化不变性一直没有得到充分的研究。这项研究测试了三个集体主义国家的IDEA-亚美尼亚,中国,和俄罗斯。样本包括18至29岁的868名学生(总男性-152名,总女性-716名)。我们在这三个国家分别测试了问卷,以检查该模型是否符合,但我们没能证明.之后,我们使用因素分析方法为这三个国家找到了一个共同的版本。我们根据理论得到了一个五因素相关模型,但它从31个项目减少到21个,三个项目转移到其他因素。最后,我们提供了测量不变性,并达到了配置水平。为了测试因子的较窄方面,我们使用了多组对齐,发现六个参数的方差不同,主要是不稳定。尽管问卷项目有所不同,我们提出了三个国家的通用模型,我们称之为问卷IDEA-集体主义国家(IDEA-CC)。
    Emerging adulthood is the youth trajectory characterized by self-focus, identity exploration, feeling between adolescence and adulthood, instability, and experimentation. This trajectory was first identified in industrialized individualistic countries with gender equality and technological progress. To measure transition to adulthood, the Inventory of the Dimensions of Emerging Adulthood (IDEA) was created. Although emerging adulthood is considered universal, adaptations of the questionnaire across the 12 countries show different patterns, and its cross-cultural invariance has been underinvestigated. This study tests IDEA in three collectivistic countries - Armenia, China, and Russia. The sample consisted of 868 students (total male - 152, total female - 716) aged 18 to 29 years old. We tested the questionnaire separately in the three countries to check that this model fits, but we failed to prove it. After that we used a factor-analytic approach to find a common version for the three countries. We got a five-factor correlated model in accordance with the theory, but it was reduced from 31 items to 21, and three items moved to other factors. Finally, we provided measurement invariance and reached configural level. To test the narrower facets of factors we used multi-group alignment and found that variances in six parameters differ, mainly in Instability. Despite the difference in the questionnaire items, we proposed a common model for three countries that we called questionnaire IDEA-collectivistic countries (IDEA-CC).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自传记忆和个人生活故事通常被认为是关于自我的记忆。然而,个人生活故事往往包含来自他人生活的重要事件的信息。有时这些记忆会成为我们生活故事的重要组成部分,阐明其他人在回忆我们个人过去时所扮演的角色。在这项研究中,我们检查了对重要生活故事事件的记忆自我聚焦的程度(例如,我搬到了日本)或其他专注(例如,我的孩子大学毕业)。来自墨西哥的与会者,格陵兰,中国,丹麦和美国回顾并确定了七个重要的个人生活故事事件的自传记忆。参与者还对记忆的重要性和情绪效价进行了评分。记忆被编码为自我或其他焦点。参与者回忆主要是以自我为中心的记忆。然而,丹麦和中国参与者回忆了大约20%的其他记忆。丹麦参与者回忆起他们父母的负面事件,而中国参与者回忆起他们孩子的积极事件。以自我为中心的记忆和以其他为中心的记忆在情感效价和寿命分布上有所不同,但不重要。研究结果表明,个人会记住其他重点记忆,并将其纳入自己的个人生活故事中。讨论了自传记忆的概念含义。
    Autobiographical memory and personal life stories are typically conceived as memories about the self. However, personal life stories often contain information about important events from other people\'s lives. Sometimes those memories become an important part of our own life stories, illuminating the role that other people play in remembering our personal past. In this study, we examined the extent to which memories of important life story events are self-focused (e.g., I moved to Japan) or other-focused (e.g., My child graduated from college). Participants from Mexico, Greenland, China, Denmark and the United States recalled and dated seven autobiographical memories of important personal life story events. Participants also rated the memories for importance and emotional valence. The memories were coded as self- or other-focused. Participants recalled mainly self-focused memories. However, Danish and Chinese participants recalled about 20% other-focused memories. Danish participants recalled negative events about their parents, whereas Chinese participants recalled positive events about their children. Self-focused and other-focused memories differed in their emotional valence and lifespan distribution, but not in importance. The findings show that individuals remember other-focused memories and also incorporate them into their own personal life stories. Conceptual implications for autobiographical memory are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管研究结果表明,抑郁症患者的个人记忆以稀疏的情节细节为特征,在某些情况下,相反的模式出现了。具体来说,最近的一项研究(Salmon等人。,2021)表明,对于社区青年来说,在高度自我相关的叙述(人生转折点)中更多的情节细节预测抑郁症状同时和一年后增加。在一项针对年轻人的新纵向研究中(时间1时N=320,M=16.9岁;81%为女性),随访6个月以上,我们旨在复制和扩展这一发现。在研究A中,我们将转折点与关于冲突事件的叙述进行了比较,确定转折点记忆中的细节是否唯一地预测了抑郁症状。支持第一个假设,在这两个时间点,更多的情节细节与抑郁症状同时呈正相关,仅在转折点叙述中.与我们的第二个假设相反,更多细节并不能纵向预测抑郁症状增加.反向模式很明显,然而,在六个月后的转折点叙述中,更大的初始抑郁症状预测了更多的细节。在研究B中,我们确定,在转折点(而非冲突事件)中,情景细节和抑郁症状之间的并发关联因自我聚焦的语言标记(I-talk更大,距离更低)而加剧.这些发现表明,当年轻人以高度的自我专注来叙述经历时,转折点叙事中的更多细节可能独特地表示心理困扰的风险。
    Although research findings show that the personal memories of people who are depressed are characterized by sparse episodic detail, under some circumstances, the opposite pattern emerges. Specifically, a recent study (Salmon et al., 2021) has shown that for community youth, greater episodic detail in a highly self-relevant narrative (a life turning point) predicted increased depressive symptoms concurrently and one year later. In a new longitudinal study of young people (N = 320 at Time 1, M = 16.9 years; 81% female) followed up over six months, we aimed to replicate and extend this finding. In Study A, we compared the turning point with a narrative about a conflict event, to establish whether the detail in a turning point memory uniquely predicted depressive symptoms. Supporting the first hypothesis, at both time-points, greater episodic detail was concurrently positively associated with depressive symptoms for turning point narratives only. Contrary to our second hypothesis, greater detail did not predict increased depressive symptoms longitudinally. The reverse pattern was significant, however, in that greater initial depressive symptoms predicted greater detail uniquely in the turning point narrative six months later. In Study B, we determined that the concurrent association between episodic detail and depressive symptoms in turning points (but not conflict events) was exacerbated by linguistic markers of self-focus (greater I-talk and lower distancing language). These findings suggest that greater detail in a turning point narrative may uniquely signify risk of psychological distress when youth narrate the experience with heightened self-focus.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在情景记忆中,旧的/新的影响,由正确识别的研究项目和正确拒绝的新项目引起的波形对比,受到广泛关注。然而,自引用编码对源内存中的旧/新效果的贡献(即,source-SRE),远未澄清;此外,目前尚不清楚这种贡献是否容易受到刺激情绪因素的影响。为了解决这些问题,采用事件相关电位(ERP)技术,这项研究应用了三种情绪效价的词(积极,中性,vs.负)在自我聚焦与外部焦点编码任务。在测试过程中,确定了四个ERP旧/新效应:(a)熟悉和回忆反映的中额叶效应(FN400)和晚期正成分(LPC)均与源SRE和刺激情绪无关;(b)重建驱动的晚期后验负(LL)表现出源SRE的不利模式,并且通过编码焦点易受情绪效价PN的影响;(c)右额叶/新效应(RFE)反映检索后过程,在情感话语中表现出来源SRE。这些效应为刺激效价和编码焦点对源存储器中SRE的影响提供了令人信服的证据,尤其是在后期过程中。提出了考虑更多观点的进一步方向。
    In episodic memory, the old/new effect, the contrast of the waveforms elicited by the correctly recognized studied items and the correctly rejected novel items, has been broadly concerned. However, the contribution of self-referential encoding to the old/new effect in source memory (i.e., source-SRE), is far from clarification; further, it remains unclear whether the contribution is susceptible to the factor of stimulus emotionality. To address these issues, adopting the event-related potential (ERP) technique, this study applied words of three types of emotional valences (positive, neutral, vs. negative) in the self-focus vs. external-focus encoding tasks. In the course of the test, four ERP old/new effects were identified: (a) the familiarity- and recollection-reflected mid-frontal effect (FN400) and late positive component (LPC) were both independent of source-SRE and stimulus emotionality; (b) the reconstruction-driven late posterior negativity (LPN) exhibited an adverse pattern of source-SRE and was susceptible to the emotional valence by encoding focus; and (c) the right frontal old/new effect (RFE), reflecting post-retrieval process, exhibited a source-SRE in emotional words. These effects provide compelling evidence for the influences of both stimulus valence and encoding focus on SRE in source memory, especially during the late processes. Further directions considering more perspectives are put forward.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    未经证实:报告性身份不确定的人有大量饮酒和饮酒障碍症状的风险。当前的研究调查了成年早期具有不同性别身份不确定性(N=75)的女性样本中,厌恶自我专注状态对随后食用表面上含酒精的饮料的影响。
    UNASSIGNED:利用2(自聚焦:负对中性)×2(任何心理不适的归因:外部与Nonegiven)between-subjectsdesignwith3within-personassessmentsofsalitary皮质醇,同时检验了适度模型和混合效应一般线性模型。
    UNASSIGNED:厌恶自我集中的状态导致女性的整体消费增加,女性的性别身份不确定性更高。研究结果表明,在性身份不确定性较高的女性中,更有可能饮用表面上含酒精的饮料,她们还报告说,她们也饮用饮料来应对痛苦。在报告性身份不确定性和饮酒应对动机较高的女性中,唾液皮质醇浓度随着时间的推移下降得更快,因为他们应该饮酒。
    未经批准:研究结果表明,在报告性身份不确定性的女性中,她们有动力饮酒以忘记麻烦或担忧,引起厌恶自我集中状态的情况可能会导致成年早期饮酒的差异。
    UNASSIGNED: Individuals who report sexual identity-uncertainty are at-risk for heavy alcohol consumption and alcohol use disorder symptomology. The current study examined the impact of states of aversive self-focus on subsequent consumption of ostensibly alcohol-containing beverages among a sample of women in early adulthood with varying levels of sexual identity-uncertainty (N = 75).
    UNASSIGNED: Utilizing a 2 (self-focus: negative vs. neutral) × 2 (attribution for any psychological discomfort: external vs. none given) between-subjects design with 3 within-person assessments of salivary cortisol, both a moderation model and mixed-effects general linear model were tested.
    UNASSIGNED: States of aversive self-focus caused increases in overall consumption among women higher in sexual identity-uncertainty. Findings suggested consumption of ostensibly alcohol-containing beverages was more likely among women higher in sexual identity-uncertainty who also reported consuming beverages to cope with distress. Among women who reported higher levels of sexual identity-uncertainty and drinking-to-cope motives, salivary cortisol concentrations dampened more quickly over time, as they supposedly consumed alcohol.
    UNASSIGNED: Findings demonstrate that, among women reporting sexual identity-uncertainty who are motivated to consume alcohol to forget about troubles or worries, situations which evoke states of aversive self-focus may contribute to differences in alcohol consumption in early adulthood.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    足球的点球可以说是世界上最引人注目和最高压的运动场合之一。这项研究检查了高压协议对罚球表现的影响,以及相关的心理和心理生理反应,以及对该协议的看法。采用顺序混合方法研究设计,其中实验条件包括以压力为参与者内部因素的重复测量设计(低压,高压)。定量数据收集得到了半结构化访谈的补充。20名职业足球运动员参加了比赛。在这两种情况之前,给予即时焦虑测量量表(IAMS)和压力Likert型量表。NeXus-10生物反馈系统记录了运动员的心率和呼吸率。外场球员对四个目标进行了五次点球大战,在低压和高压下。压力,认知焦虑,在高压条件下,呼吸速率显着增加。在控制高压条件下的压力水平后,双变量变量误差性能显著提高(即,球员在压力下的投篮分组变化更大)。定性数据表明,六步高压方案主要引起了分心机制。这项研究为模拟高压罚球协议提供了对职业足球运动员经验的初步见解。
    The penalty kick in soccer is arguably one of the most highly visible and high-pressured sporting situations in the world. This study examined the effect of a high-pressure protocol on penalty shooting performance, and the associated psychological and psychophysiological responses to, and perceptions of that protocol. A sequential mixed-method research design was employed wherein  the experimental condition consisted of a repeated-measures design with pressure as the within-participants factor (low-pressure, high-pressure). Quantitative data collection was complemented with a semi-structured interview. Twenty professional footballers took part. The Immediate Anxiety Measurement Scale (IAMS) and Pressure Likert-type scale were administered prior to both conditions. The NeXus-10 biofeedback system recorded players\' heart rate and respiration rate. Outfield players took five penalty kicks to four targets to the goal, under low and high-pressure. Pressure, cognitive anxiety, and respiration rate significantly increased in the high-pressure condition. After controlling for the level of pressure experienced in the high-pressure condition, bivariate variable error performance significantly increased (i.e., players were more variable in their grouping of shots under stress). Qualitative data indicated the 6-step high-pressure protocol predominantly evoked the distraction mechanism. This research provided first insight into the experiences of professional footballers to a simulated high-pressure penalty protocol.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    过去的研究表明,抑郁个体比非抑郁个体不太可能对负面情绪进行情绪不一致的回忆,并且不会经历相关的情绪修复效果。本研究研究了在抑郁症状不同的个体中,对一个人的情绪采用反思性和反思性的自我聚焦取向对负面情绪诱导后回忆的自传记忆的效价以及记忆后情绪修复的程度的影响。参与者经历了负面的情绪诱导和反思(n=69)或反思(n=49)的自我聚焦操纵,然后回忆了五个特定的自传记忆。抑郁症状与回忆较少的积极记忆和情绪修复减少有关。回忆记忆的效价与情绪改善的程度有关,而抑郁症状并未缓解这种关联.与我们的假设相反,与反思性自我聚焦相比,反思性自我聚焦与回忆更多的积极记忆或更大的情绪改善无关.结果表明,在消极情绪状态下,更多抑郁的个体不太可能自发地进行情绪不一致的回忆;然而,积极记忆的回忆与相似的情绪修复效果相关,而与抑郁症状无关。
    Past research suggests that depressed individuals are less likely than non-depressed individuals to engage in mood-incongruent recall in response to negative mood and do not experience associated mood reparative effects. The present study examined the effects of adopting a reflective versus ruminative self-focus orientation towards one\'s mood on the valence of autobiographical memories recalled following a negative mood induction and the extent of mood repair following memory recall among individuals with varying depressive symptomatology. Participants underwent a negative mood induction and either a ruminative (n = 69) or reflective (n = 49) self-focus manipulation, and then recalled five specific autobiographical memories. Depression symptoms were associated with recall of less positive memories and reduced mood repair. The valence of recalled memories was associated with the extent of mood improvement, and depressive symptoms did not moderate this association. Contrary to our hypothesis, a reflective self-focus was not associated with recall of more positive memories or greater mood improvement than a ruminative self-focus. The results suggest that more depressed individuals are less likely to spontaneously engage in mood-incongruent recall in a negative mood state; however, recall of positive memories is associated with similar mood reparative effects regardless of depressive symptomatology.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    抑郁和焦虑问题在早期父母中很常见,会对父母的养育质量产生不利影响(1)。沉思与不良福祉密切相关(2),表明自我聚焦可能是幸福和护理之间联系的一个中介者[例如,(3)].在一项关于第一次为母亲和父亲的国际研究中框架(4),目前的研究包括396名英国母亲和父亲(198名异性恋同居夫妻关系)的第一个出生的4个月大婴儿.父母报告了他们的抑郁症状,他们夫妻关系中的焦虑和满足。五分钟的语音样本被转录和编码,供父母代词使用(即,\“I\”以及包含婴儿或合作伙伴的\“We\”的使用),而静脸范式中的观察结果是针对父母对婴儿线索的敏感性进行编码的。我们的第一个目标是测试新父母的自我专注是否与幸福和夫妻关系质量有关。我们还研究了(i)自我聚焦是否介导了健康与护理敏感性之间的预期关联,以及(ii)夫妻关系质量是否调节了自我聚焦与护理者敏感性之间的预期关联。最后,我们比较了母亲和父亲的结果。我们的结果说明了针对性别的关联。首先,尽管母亲和父亲的自我聚焦和包容伴侣的谈话平均水平相似,母亲对婴儿包容性使用“我们”代词的比例高于父亲。第二,自我聚焦与母亲或父亲的幸福无关,但与父亲关于夫妻关系质量下降的报告有关。此外,不良的围产期健康状况与父亲的伴侣包容性谈话减少有关,但减少了母亲对婴儿包容性谈话的使用。第三,调解模型表明,婴儿包容性谈话的减少是母亲幸福感差与敏感性降低之间联系的基础,但不是父亲。第四,在良好的夫妻关系质量的背景下,母亲对伴侣包容性谈话的增加与护理敏感性降低相关。这些发现是根据它们对支持新父母的干预措施的影响进行讨论的,他们可能会受益于不同的策略,以培养对他们发育中的婴儿的关注。
    Problems of depression and anxiety are common in early parenthood and adversely affect parenting quality (1). Rumination is closely linked to poor wellbeing (2), suggesting that self-focus may be one mediator of the association between wellbeing and caregiving [e.g., (3)]. Framed within an international study of first-time mothers and fathers (4), the current study included 396 British mothers and fathers (in 198 heterosexual cohabiting couple relationships) of first-born 4-month-old infants. Parents reported on their symptoms of depression, anxiety and satisfaction in their couple relationship. Five-minute speech samples were transcribed and coded for parents\' pronoun use (i.e., \"I\" and either infant- or partner-inclusive use of \"We\"), whilst observations in the Still-Face paradigm were coded for parental sensitivity to infants\' cues. Our first goal was to test whether new parents\' self-focus was associated with wellbeing and couple relationship quality. We also examined whether (i) self-focus mediated the expected association between wellbeing and caregiving sensitivity and (ii) couple relationship quality moderated the expected association between self-focus and caregiver sensitivity. Finally, we compared results for mothers and fathers. Our results illustrate gender-specific associations. First, although mean levels of self-focus and partner-inclusive talk were similar for mothers and fathers, infant-inclusive use of the \"we\" pronoun was higher in mothers than fathers. Second, self-focus was unrelated to either mothers\' or fathers\' wellbeing, but was associated with fathers\' report of reduced couple relationship quality. In addition, poor perinatal wellbeing was associated with reduced partner-inclusive talk for fathers, but with reduced use of infant-inclusive talk for mothers. Third, mediation models suggest that reduced infant-inclusive talk underpins the association between poor wellbeing and reduced sensitivity in mothers, but not fathers. Fourth, in the context of good couple relationship quality, mothers\' elevated partner-inclusive talk was associated with reduced caregiving sensitivity. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for interventions to support new mothers and fathers, who may benefit from distinct strategies to foster attention to their developing infant.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Most of our knowledge related to how social exclusion affects those who ostracize and those who are being ostracized is based on questionnaires administered after the ostracism situation is over. In this research, we strived to further our understanding of the internal dynamics of an ostracism situation. We therefore examined individuals\' language-specifically, function words-as a behavior indicative of psychological processes and emergent states that can be unobtrusively recorded right in the situation. In online chats, 128 participants talked about a personal topic in groups of three. In the experimental group (n = 79), two conversation partners ignored every contribution by the third. We found that, compared to the control group, these targets of ostracism used language indicative of a self-focus and worsened mood, but not of social focus or positivity, although positivity was related to a writer\'s likeability. Sources of ostracism used language suggesting that they were distancing themselves from the situation, and they further engaged in victim derogation. We discuss how our results highlight the severity and potential self-sustainability of ostracism.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的研究报告说,模仿反应可能受到自上而下的社会因素的调节,如自我聚焦。然而,越来越多的证据表明,这些社会因素实际上可能会调节领域的一般过程,如空间相容的反应,而不是具体的社会过程,如模仿。在这项研究中,我们的目的是确定在自我集中增强或减弱的情况下被调节的认知过程。参与者执行了刺激-反应兼容性任务,该任务独立测量空间和模仿反应倾向,在两个条件下:自我聚焦增强,在有两个镜子的情况下执行任务;自我聚焦减弱,镜子被遮住的地方。虽然参与者对兼容试验的反应比对不兼容试验的反应更快,无论是模仿还是空间,自聚焦对空间相容性或模仿相容性没有明显的调制;尽管空间相容性调制的大小在数值上与以前研究中发现的自聚焦对模仿的影响相似。这些结果没有提供证据表明自我专注对任何一种社会特定的影响,或通用域,进程。
    Previous studies have reported that imitative responses may be modulated by top-down social factors such as self-focus. However, growing evidence suggests that such social factors may actually modulate domain-general processes such as spatially compatible responding, rather than specifically social processes such as imitation. In this study, we aimed to identify the cognitive processes being modulated under conditions of heightened or diminished self-focus. Participants performed a stimulus-response compatibility task which independently measures both spatial and imitative response tendencies, under two conditions: heightened self-focus, where the task was performed in the presence of two mirrors; and diminished self-focus, where the mirrors were covered. While participants were faster to respond to compatible trials than to incompatible trials, both imitatively and spatially, there was no significant modulation of either spatial or imitative compatibility by self-focus; although the magnitude of the modulation of spatial compatibility was numerically similar to the effect of self-focus on imitation found in previous studies. These results provide no evidence for an effect of self-focus on either social-specific, or domain-general, processes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号