Recall

召回
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    个人可以在对话中扮演各种角色。有些角色更活跃,参与者负责指导对话以追求小组的目标。其他角色更被动,就像一个人是一个疏忽的人。经典说法认为,过度听众不会形成对话的共同点,因为他们没有积极参与交流过程。的确,实证研究结果表明,过度听众并不像积极的参与者那样理解对话。鲜为人知,然而,关于谈话过度者的长期记忆。在法律环境和争议解决中,过问者发挥着重要作用。了解他们的记忆在质量和内容上与谈话中的活跃参与者有何不同是至关重要的。在这里,我们第一次研究了作为演讲者的对话角色的影响,收件人,或在随后的对话记忆中被忽略。60名参与者回忆60次对话的数据显示,在短暂的延迟之后,与说话者和收件人相比,过度听众从对话中回忆起的内容要少得多,他们回忆的内容不太准确地来源于其实际贡献者。主动对话参与者之间的助记符相似性高于主动参与者和过度听众之间的相似性。这些发现为以下假设提供了关键支持:在交互式对话中形成共同点的过程形成并支持该对话的记忆。
    Individuals can take on various roles in conversation. Some roles are more active, with the participant responsible for guiding that conversation in pursuit of the group\'s goals. Other roles are more passive, like when one is an overhearer. Classic accounts posit that overhearers do not form conversational common ground because they do not actively participate in the communication process. Indeed, empirical findings demonstrate that overhearers do not comprehend conversation as well as active participants. Little is known, however, about long-term memory for conversations in overhearers. Overhearers play an important role in legal settings and dispute resolution, and it is critical to understand how their memory differs in quality and content from active participants in conversation. Here we examine - for the first time - the impact of one\'s conversational role as a speaker, addressee, or overhearer on subsequent memory for conversation. Data from 60 participants recalling 60 conversations reveal that after a brief delay, overhearers recall significantly less content from conversation compared to both speakers and addressees, and that the content they do recall is less accurately sourced to its actual contributor. Mnemonic similarity is higher between active conversational participants than between active participants and overhearers. These findings provide key support for the hypothesis that the process of forming common ground in interactive conversation shapes and supports memory for that conversation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    越来越多的证据表明,错误记忆可能发生在工作记忆(WM)任务中,在学习和测试之间只有几个语义相关的单词和几秒钟。Abadie和Camos(2019)提出了一种新模型,通过描述发音排练和注意力刷新的作用来解释错误记忆的形成,WM中主动维护信息的两种主要机制。然而,该模型仅在识别任务中进行了测试。在本研究中,我们报告了在召回任务中测试模型的四个实验,其中WM中信息的主动维护在检索中起着更重要的作用。在较短的保留间隔内保留了与语义相关的项目的简短列表,其中充满了并发任务,该任务要么损害了每个WM维护机制的使用,要么不使用。参与者被要求在并发任务(即时测试)后立即或稍后回忆项目,在几组试验结束时(延迟试验)。在即时测试中,当WM维护受损时,语义错误更频繁。具体来说,排练防止了即时测试中语义错误的发生,虽然在这个测试中清爽对它们的发生没有影响,但增加的语义错误仅在延迟测试中产生。这些结果支持Abadie和Camos(2019)模型,并进一步证明了WM中主动信息维护在错误记忆出现中的作用。讨论了这些发现对理解WM-LTM关系的意义。
    There is growing evidence that false memories can occur in working memory (WM) tasks with only a few semantically related words and seconds between study and test. Abadie and Camos (2019) proposed a new model to explain the formation of false memories by describing the role of articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing, the two main mechanisms for actively maintaining information in WM. However, this model has only been tested in recognition tasks. In the present study, we report four experiments testing the model in recall tasks in which the active maintenance of information in WM plays a more important role for retrieval. Short lists of semantically related items were held for a short retention interval filled with a concurrent task that either impaired or not the use of each of the WM maintenance mechanisms. Participants were asked to recall the items immediately after the concurrent task (immediate test) or later, at the end of a block of several trials (delayed test). In the immediate test, semantic errors were more frequent when WM maintenance was impaired. Specifically, rehearsal prevented the occurrence of semantic errors in the immediate test, while refreshing had no effect on their occurrence in this test, but increased semantic errors produced only in the delayed test. These results support Abadie and Camos (2019) model and go further by demonstrating the role of active information maintenance in WM in the emergence of false memories. The implications of these findings for understanding WM-LTM relationships are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当我们驾驭日常生活时,我们经常根据我们预测在给定环境中接下来可能发生的事情来调整我们的行为。当意外事件发生时,我们对世界的预测被打乱了,必须更新。意想不到的,孤立事件,特别是高情感,也更好地召回。在目前的工作中,我们研究了古怪球如何影响召回动态。七十年轻,健康的参与者在不同的刺激开始异步时编码单词列表,其中包含情感或感知的怪球,然后是自由回忆。众所周知,在回忆一件物品后,我们回忆附近编码的物品的概率更高,特别是那些在项目被召回后编码的,一种被称为召回前向连续性的现象。我们测试了新颖性(怪球与控制词)如何根据显着性类型(情感与感知)调节前向连续性。本结果提供了通过情绪显著性选择性地进行前向连续性调节的经验证据,并表明在表现出情绪和知觉怪球后的回忆模式是由不同的机制介导的。
    As we navigate our day-to-day lives, we regularly adapt our behaviour according to what we predict may happen next in a given context. When an unexpected event occurs, our predictions about the world are disrupted and must be updated. Unexpected, isolated events, particularly with high emotionality, are also better recalled. In the present work, we investigated how oddballs affect recall dynamics. Seventy young, healthy participants encoded word lists containing either emotional or perceptual oddballs at varying stimulus onset asynchronies followed by free recall. It is well established that after recalling an item, we have a higher probability of recalling items encoded nearby, particularly those that were encoded after the item was recalled, a phenomenon known as forward contiguity of recall. We tested how novelty (oddballs versus control words) modulated forward contiguity as a function of salience type (emotional versus perceptual). The present results provide empirical evidence of forward contiguity modulation selectively by emotional salience and suggest that recall patterns after presenting emotional and perceptual oddballs are mediated by different mechanisms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:辐射回忆反应(RRR)是在触发剂之后在先前照射的区域中发生的罕见炎症反应。在儿科患者中,人们对它知之甚少,研究不足。吉西他滨-多西他赛(G/D)在儿童癌症中主要用作肉瘤的挽救方案。我们旨在描述儿童G/D引发的RRR。
    方法:回顾性回顾了2010年至2022年在两家医院接受G/D同时放疗的21例患者。RRR被认为是在先前照射的区域中G/D给药后发生的任何毒性。描述了RRR特征。Fisher和Mann-Whitney检验用于分析涉及的危险因素。
    结果:16例(76.2%)患者发生16次RRR发作。RRR主要涉及皮肤深层(58%),主要发生在两个G/D周期后。放疗和化疗的平均时间为28.5天(0-1359天),和RRR的平均放射量391mL(157-1810mL)。RRR治疗主要是全身性类固醇,11例患者中有6例(58%)有部分反应。再次暴露于G/D与15人中有9人的高复发率(56.2%)相关,提示药物停药。G/D后RRR的主要危险因素包括,没有统计学意义,照射场的体积更大,化疗和放疗之间的间隔更短。
    结论:儿童人群G/D后RRR的发生率高于以前的报道。药物再次暴露后通常会复发。较高的照射量和较短的化疗开始时间可能与RRR的风险增加有关。
    BACKGROUND: Radiation recall reaction (RRR) is a rare inflammatory reaction developing in a previously irradiated field after a triggering agent. In pediatric patients, it is poorly understood and deficiently studied. Gemcitabine-docetaxel (G/D) in childhood cancer is mainly used as a salvage regimen for sarcomas. We aim to describe RRR triggered by G/D in children.
    METHODS: Retrospective review of 21 patients receiving G/D along with radiotherapy at two hospitals from 2010 until 2022. RRR was considered as any toxicity occurring after G/D administration in a previously irradiated field. RRR features were described. Fisher\'s and Mann-Whitney tests were utilized to analyze the risk factors involved.
    RESULTS: Sixteen episodes of RRR developed in 16 (76.2%) patients. RRR mainly involved deep layers of the skin (58%) and occurred predominantly after two G/D cycles. The mean time between radiotherapy and chemotherapy was 28.5 days (0-1359 days), and the mean radiation volume 391 mL (157-1810 mL) for RRR. RRR treatment was mainly systemic steroids, with partial responses in six of 11 (58%) patients. Re-exposure to G/D was associated with a high rate of recurrence in nine of 15 (56.2%), prompting drug discontinuation. The major risk factors for RRR after G/D include, without statistical significance, a larger volume of the irradiated field and a shorter interval between chemotherapy and radiotherapy.
    CONCLUSIONS: The incidence of RRR after G/D in the pediatric population is higher than previously reported. Drug re-exposure is usually followed by recurrence. Higher irradiated volumes and a shorter time to the start of chemotherapy could be related with an increased risk of RRR.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    动机和情绪对记忆的影响已被广泛研究;然而,尽管这些结构之间有联系,它们已经在不同的研究领域进行了研究,很少有工作同时检查它们的效果。当前的研究采用了一种新颖的方法来正交地操纵动机和情绪影响,在同样的任务中,来测试它们在有意记忆形成上的相互作用。如果情感和奖励动机紧密相连,它们可能依赖于重叠的认知机制,因此,我们不会期望情感和奖励在记忆中相互作用。或者,它们可能是不同的结构,因此当两者都包含在同一个实验试验中时,会增强记忆,超越加性效应。为了测试这些相互竞争的预测,在实验1中,参与者(n=180)以情绪(消极,积极)和中性词故意编码高或低奖励预期线索。在实验2中,参与者(n=159)用高或低的奖励提示编码情感和中性词,但是记忆是使用研究测试块免费回忆测试的。两个实验的结果都是一致的。情绪和奖励在一般假设的方向上有主要影响,但没有证据表明这些因素之间存在相互作用。这与情感和奖励动机是相似结构的预测是一致的。在试验中,它们的组合并不能单独增强这些因素中的任何一个,这可能表明这些结构具有相似的认知机制。
    Motivational and emotional influences on memory have been studied extensively; however, despite the link between these constructs, they have been studied in separate lines of research, with very little work examining their effects concurrently. The current study takes a novel approach to manipulate motivational and emotional influences orthogonally, and within the same task, to test their interplay on intentional memory formation. If emotion and reward motivation are tightly linked, they may rely on overlapping cognitive mechanisms, thus we would not expect emotion and reward to interact in memory. Alternatively, they could be distinct constructs and therefore would boost memory when both are included in the same experimental trial, above and beyond additive effects. To test these competing predictions, in Experiment 1, participants (n = 180) completed an old/new recognition memory task with emotional (negative, positive) and neutral words intentionally encoded with high or low reward anticipation cues. In Experiment 2, participants (n = 159) encoded emotional and neutral words with a high or low reward cue, but memory was tested with free recall using study-test blocks. The findings from both experiments converged. There were main effects of emotion and reward in generally hypothesized directions, but no evidence of an interaction between these factors. This is in line with the prediction that emotion and reward motivation are similar constructs. Their combination within a trial does not boost memory above and beyond either of these factors alone perhaps indicating these constructs have similar cognitive mechanisms.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    记忆不仅是为了个人回忆而存储的,而且要向他人传达知识,为适应性决策服务。先前的研究表明,共享信息的目标可以改变在内存中传达的内容以及嵌入在此沟通中的语言风格。然而,鲜为人知的是,与通信相关的记忆叙述变化如何驱动听众的价值处理差异。这里,我们测试了记忆通信如何改变复杂事件的多特征回忆,以及对幼稚听众的价值估计的下游后果。参与者回忆起在24小时的延迟下玩探索性视频游戏的记忆,以分享(即,社会条件)或回忆(即,控制条件)他们的记忆。共享目标系统地改变了回忆的内容和语言风格,因此,来自社会条件的叙述者偏向于回忆非情节细节,并以更大的影响力传达他们的记忆,少了一些形式,更少的真实性。在两个独立的天真听众样本中,这些特征不同地影响了视频游戏的价值估计。我们发现,更大的影响力与更多的享受相关,同时聆听记忆(享乐价值),并且更多地包含非情节细节导致更大的意愿购买视频游戏(动机驱动)。这些发现表明,作为故事分享经历可以改变记忆回忆的内容和语言基调,这反过来又塑造了天真听众的感知价值。
    Memories are not only stored for personal recall, but also to communicate knowledge to others in service of adaptive decision-making. Prior research shows that goals to share information can change which content is communicated in memory as well as the linguistic style embedded in this communication. Yet, little is known as to how communication-related alterations in memory narration drive differences of value processing in listeners. Here, we test how memory communication alters multi-featural recall for complex events and the downstream consequence on value estimations in naïve listeners. Participants recalled a memory of playing an exploratory videogame at a 24-h delay under instructions to either share (i.e., social condition) or recall (i.e., control condition) their memory. Sharing goals systematically altered the content and linguistic style of recall, such that narrators from the social condition were biased towards recall of non-episodic details and communicated their memories with more clout, less formality, and less authenticity. Across two independent samples of naïve listeners, these features differentially influenced value estimations of the video game. We found that greater clout was associated with greater enjoyment while listening to memories (hedonic value), and that greater inclusion of non-episodic details resulted in greater willingness to purchase the video game (motivational drive). These findings indicate that sharing an experience as a story can change the content and linguistic tone of memory recall, which in turn shape perceived value in naïve listeners.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在本研究中,我们测试了在祖先生存情景中处理信息是否能提高老年人和卒中患者的情景记忆能力.在一项在线研究(实验1)中,健康的年轻人和老年人根据单词与祖先生存场景的相关性对单词进行评级,并将随后的自由召回性能与主题内设计中的愉悦判断任务和移动场景任务进行了比较。复制了典型的生存处理效果:生存任务中的召回率最高,接下来是移动和愉快的判断任务。尽管老年人的整体召回率较低,没有证据表明不同年龄组的病情影响存在差异.实验2是在神经康复诊所中进行的,样本是在过去5个月内患有中风的患者。在团体层面,实验2显示,三种条件之间的召回率没有显着差异。然而,当考虑整体记忆能力和执行功能时,在标准化神经心理学测试中独立测量,患者表现出显著的生存处理效果。此外,只有执行功能得分高的患者才能从情景任务中受益,这表明完整的执行功能可能是记忆益处所必需的。一起来看,我们的结果支持这样的观点,即生存处理任务-实验心理学领域的一项经过充分研究的任务-可以被纳入到补偿记忆功能障碍的策略中.
    In the present study, we tested whether processing information in the context of an ancestral survival scenario enhances episodic memory performance in older adults and in stroke patients. In an online study (Experiment 1), healthy young and older adults rated words according to their relevance to an ancestral survival scenario, and subsequent free recall performance was compared to a pleasantness judgment task and a moving scenario task in a within-subject design. The typical survival processing effect was replicated: Recall rates were highest in the survival task, followed by the moving and the pleasantness judgment task. Although older adults showed overall lower recall rates, there was no evidence for differences between the age groups in the condition effects. Experiment 2 was conducted in a neurological rehabilitation clinic with a sample of patients who had suffered from a stroke within the past 5 months. On the group level, Experiment 2 revealed no significant difference in recall rates between the three conditions. However, when accounting for overall memory abilities and executive function, independently measured in standardized neuropsychological tests, patients showed a significant survival processing effect. Furthermore, only patients with high executive function scores benefitted from the scenario tasks, suggesting that intact executive function may be necessary for a mnemonic benefit. Taken together, our results support the idea that the survival processing task - a well-studied task in the field of experimental psychology - may be incorporated into a strategy to compensate for memory dysfunction.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Engram标签和操作方法现在是当代神经科学实践的主要内容,给人的印象是已经发现了encrams的物理基础。尽管取得了巨大进展,电报还没有被清楚地识别,目前还不清楚它们应该是什么样子。Engram神经科学中存在一种认识偏见,倾向于表征生物学变化,而忽略了理论的发展。然而,Engram生物学的工具之所以令人兴奋,正是因为它们不仅是理解可塑性和学习机制的一个渐进的进步,而且因为它们可以被用来为神经科学中的一个基本谜题提供理论信息--大脑如何以及以什么格式存储信息。我们在这里不提出这样的理论,因为我们首先需要对缺乏的东西有一个赞赏。我们在四个部分中概述了理论生物学和哲学中的一系列问题,这些问题通常应结合生物学和系统神经科学,以构建有用的未来理论框架。具体来说,应该解释什么?全脑全图的不同构建块是如何融合在一起的?这些组成部分到底是什么?它们携带了什么信息,问这些问题不仅仅是哲学的特权,而是告知科学假设的关键,这些假设充分利用了我们可以使用的实验工具。不处理这些问题的风险很高。如果没有关于恩谱是什么的理论,他们做什么,以及它们适合的更广泛的计算过程,我们可能永远不知道他们什么时候被发现。
    Engram labelling and manipulation methodologies are now a staple of contemporary neuroscientific practice, giving the impression that the physical basis of engrams has been discovered. Despite enormous progress, engrams have not been clearly identified, and it is unclear what they should look like. There is an epistemic bias in engram neuroscience toward characterizing biological changes while neglecting the development of theory. However, the tools of engram biology are exciting precisely because they are not just an incremental step forward in understanding the mechanisms of plasticity and learning but because they can be leveraged to inform theory on one of the fundamental mysteries in neuroscience-how and in what format the brain stores information. We do not propose such a theory here, as we first require an appreciation for what is lacking. We outline a selection of issues in four sections from theoretical biology and philosophy that engram biology and systems neuroscience generally should engage with in order to construct useful future theoretical frameworks. Specifically, what is it that engrams are supposed to explain? How do the different building blocks of the brain-wide engram come together? What exactly are these component parts? And what information do they carry, if they carry anything at all? Asking these questions is not purely the privilege of philosophy but a key to informing scientific hypotheses that make the most of the experimental tools at our disposal. The risk for not engaging with these issues is high. Without a theory of what engrams are, what they do, and the wider computational processes they fit into, we may never know when they have been found.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:医疗记录摘要(MRA)和自我报告问卷是确定癌症治疗信息的两种常用方法。先前的研究表明,MRA和自我报告之间有很好的一致性,但尚不清楚超过3年的召回窗口会如何影响该协议。
    方法:妇女环境癌症和辐射流行病学(WECARE)研究是一个多中心,基于人群的病例对照研究,对单侧乳腺癌对照与对侧乳腺癌病例进行单独匹配.从1985年到2008年,在55岁之前被诊断出患有首次原发性乳腺癌的参与者填写了一份问卷,其中包括有关治疗的问题。首次原发性乳腺癌治疗信息是从放射治疗的放射肿瘤学临床记录以及激素治疗和化疗的全身辅助治疗报告中提取的。使用kappa统计量和相应的95%置信区间(CI)评估MRA与自我报告治疗之间的一致性。
    结果:共有2808名患者接受MRA和自我报告的化疗治疗信息,2733名具有MRA和自我报告激素治疗信息的参与者,并确定了2,905名具有MRA和自我报告放射治疗信息的参与者.召回窗口中位数为12.5年(范围,2.8-22.2年)。MRA和自我报告的治疗协议在不同的治疗方式上表现优异(kappachemo,98.5;95%CI,97.9-99.2;卡帕霍姆,87.7;95%CI,85.9-89.5;kapparad,97.9;95%CI,97.0-98.7)。在召回窗口中没有异质性(chemo=.46;phorm=.40;prad=.61)。
    结论:对于诊断为乳腺癌的年轻女性,自我报告和MRA原发性乳腺癌治疗方式信息之间的一致性非常好,甚至在诊断后20年以上的召回窗口的女性中也能保持。
    BACKGROUND: Medical record abstraction (MRA) and self-report questionnaires are two methods frequently used to ascertain cancer treatment information. Prior studies have shown excellent agreement between MRA and self-report, but it is unknown how a recall window longer than 3 years may affect this agreement.
    METHODS: The Women\'s Environmental Cancer and Radiation Epidemiology (WECARE) Study is a multicenter, population-based case-control study of controls with unilateral breast cancer individually matched to cases with contralateral breast cancer. Participants who were diagnosed with a first primary breast cancer from 1985 to 2008 before the age of 55 years completed a questionnaire that included questions on treatment. First primary breast cancer treatment information was abstracted from the medical record from radiation oncology clinic notes for radiation treatment and from systemic adjuvant treatment reports for hormone therapy and chemotherapy. Agreement between MRA and self-reported treatment was assessed with the kappa statistic and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs).
    RESULTS: A total of 2808 participants with MRA and self-reported chemotherapy treatment information, 2733 participants with MRA and self-reported hormone therapy information, and 2905 participants with MRA and self-reported radiation treatment information were identified. The median recall window was 12.5 years (range, 2.8-22.2 years). MRA and self-reported treatment agreement was excellent across treatment modalities (kappachemo, 98.5; 95% CI, 97.9-99.2; kappahorm, 87.7; 95% CI, 85.9-89.5; kapparad, 97.9; 95% CI, 97.0-98.7). There was no heterogeneity across recall windows (pchemo = .46; phorm = .40; prad = .61).
    CONCLUSIONS: Agreement between self-reported and MRA primary breast cancer treatment modality information was excellent for young women diagnosed with breast cancer and was maintained even among women whose recall window was more than 20 years after diagnosis.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号