Reaching

达到
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    到达动作可以在前进过程中重定向,以处理意外的视觉变化,例如目标位置的改变。重要的是要知道这些重定向何时开始,即在线反应时间(ORT),但这一信息并不明显,因为重定向被嵌入随时间变化的基线移动中,该移动因试验而异.先前的一项研究评估了不同的oRT识别方法的性能,利用了具有完全相同的起始点的模拟重定向,而不是通常会遇到的一系列发作。我们通过利用批次的“混合”试验解决了这一差距,这些试验在它们的oRT中具有时间传播。每个混合试验将采样的基线运动与理想化的校正响应相结合。两种新方法可以最准确地识别在线反应时间:i)阈值对齐的大均值回归和ii)基于模板的方法,我们将其称为规范校正搜索。阈值对齐的大均值回归易于实现且有效。规范校正搜索是一个更复杂的过程,但可以说与基础响应更好地联系在一起。将这两种方法应用于已发布的数据集揭示了比以前报道的更多的oRT延迟以及诸如oRT分布宽度之类的新信息。一起来看,我们的结果证明了两种新方法的实用性,可以从正在进行的运动中剖析纠正措施。
    Reaching movements can be redirected during their progress to handle unexpected visual changes, such as a change in target location. It is important to know when these redirections start, i.e. the online reaction time (oRT), but this information is not readily evident since redirections are embedded within a time-varying baseline movement which differs from trial to trial. The one previous study that evaluated the performance of different oRT identification methods utilized simulated redirections with the exact same onset, rather than a range of onsets as would be typically encountered. We addressed this gap by utilizing batches of \"hybrid\" trials with temporal spread in their oRTs. Each hybrid trial combined a sampled baseline movement with an idealized corrective response. Two new methods had the most accurate identification of online reaction times: i) a threshold-aligned grand mean regression and ii) a template-based approach we term the canonical correction search. The threshold-aligned grand mean regression is simple to implement and effective. The canonical correction search is a more complex procedure but arguably better linked to the underlying response. Applying the two methods to a published dataset revealed more delayed oRTs than was previously reported along with new information such as the width of oRT distributions. Taken together, our results demonstrate the utility of two new methods for dissecting corrective action from ongoing movement.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在包括脑瘫在内的神经发育疾病中,双侧协调通常受损,发育协调障碍,和自闭症谱系障碍。然而,我们缺乏客观的临床评估,这些评估能够以临床可行的方式量化双侧协调性,并确定基于年龄的标准来识别损伤.这项研究的目的是使用增强现实和计算机视觉来表征通常发育中的儿童的双边到达能力。通常,年龄在6-17岁的发育中的儿童(n=133)在增强现实游戏环境中完成了对称和不对称的双边延伸任务。我们分析了他们可以在50s内达到的目标对的数量以及他们的手到达目标之间的时间间隔。我们发现两个任务的性能是并行开发的,12岁后发展缓慢但没有稳定。孩子们在对称任务上比不对称任务表现更好,既达到了目标,又缩短了手差。随着年龄的增长,手滞后儿童之间的变异性降低。我们还发现性别差异,女性表现优于男性,这在10-11岁的孩子中最为明显。总的来说,这项研究表明,在儿童期和青春期,对称和不对称的伸手能力平行发展。此外,它展示了使用计算机视觉和增强现实量化双边协调的能力,可用于评估临床人群。
    Bilateral coordination is commonly impaired in neurodevelopmental conditions including cerebral palsy, developmental coordination disorder, and autism spectrum disorder. However, we lack objective clinical assessments that can quantify bilateral coordination in a clinically feasible manner and determine age-based norms to identify impairments. The objective of this study was to use augmented reality and computer vision to characterize bilateral reaching abilities in typically developing children. Typically developing children (n = 133) ages 6-17 years completed symmetric and asymmetric bilateral reaching tasks in an augmented reality game environment. We analyzed the number of target pairs they could reach in 50 s as well as the time lag between their hands reaching the targets. We found that performance on both tasks developed in parallel, with development slowing but not plateauing after age 12. Children performed better on the symmetric task than asymmetric, both in targets reached and with shorter hand lags. Variability between children in hand lag decreased with age. We also found gender differences with females outperforming males, which were most pronounced in the 10-11 year olds. Overall, this study demonstrates parallel development through childhood and adolescence of symmetric and asymmetric reaching abilities. Furthermore, it demonstrates the ability to quantify bilateral coordination using computer vision and augmented reality, which can be applied to assess clinical populations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    同位运动皮质的半球间抑制被认为对于准确的单侧运动功能是有效的。然而,单侧运动行为时半球间抑制的细胞机制尚不清楚.此外,神经调质乙酰胆碱对半球间抑制的影响和相关的细胞机制尚不清楚。为了解决这个知识差距,我们在伸爪任务期间记录了小鼠双侧运动皮层的神经元活动。随后,我们分析了细胞对水平的半球间尖峰相关性,对推定的细胞类型进行分类,以探索半球间抑制的潜在细胞回路机制。当小鼠参与到达任务时,我们发现了半球间尖峰相关性的细胞类型对特异性增强。我们还发现,药物乙酰胆碱操纵可以调节半球间的尖峰相关性。局部场对对侧激励的反应沿皮质深度不同,毒蕈碱受体拮抗作用增强了深层场反应的抑制成分。毒蕈碱型M2受体主要表达于深部皮质神经元,包括GABA能中间神经元。这些结果表明,在深层表达毒蕈碱受体的GABA能中间神经元介导了同位运动皮层半球间抑制的神经调节。
    Interhemispheric inhibition of the homotopic motor cortex is believed to be effective for accurate unilateral motor function. However, the cellular mechanisms underlying interhemispheric inhibition during unilateral motor behavior remain unclear. Furthermore, the impact of the neuromodulator acetylcholine on interhemispheric inhibition and the associated cellular mechanisms are not well understood. To address this knowledge gap, we conducted recordings of neuronal activity from the bilateral motor cortex of mice during the paw-reaching task. Subsequently, we analyzed interhemispheric spike correlation at the cell-pair level, classifying putative cell types to explore the underlying cellular circuitry mechanisms of interhemispheric inhibition. We found a cell-type pair-specific enhancement of the interhemispheric spike correlation when the mice were engaged in the reaching task. We also found that the interhemispheric spike correlation was modulated by pharmacological acetylcholine manipulation. The local field responses to contralateral excitation differed along the cortical depths, and muscarinic receptor antagonism enhanced the inhibitory component of the field response in deep layers. The muscarinic subtype M2 receptor is predominantly expressed in deep cortical neurons, including GABAergic interneurons. These results suggest that GABAergic interneurons expressing muscarinic receptors in deep layers mediate the neuromodulation of interhemispheric inhibition in the homotopic motor cortex.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在疲劳或中风等事件之后,个人经常在伸手时向前移动躯干,利用更广泛的肌肉群,即使只有手臂运动就足够了。在以前的工作中,我们展示了“部队储备”的存在-一种现象,当被沉重的重量挑战时,调整他们的运动协调,以保持大约40%的肩部力量。这里,我们调查了这种储备是否可以预测髋关节,肩膀,以及由诱发的肩部力量不足引起的肘部运动和扭矩。我们让20名健康的参与者参加了一项涉及越来越重的哑铃的任务,分析手臂和躯干的运动通过运动捕捉和关节扭矩通过逆动力学。我们使用3自由度上身的最优控制模型模拟了这些运动,对比三个成本函数:传统的平方扭矩之和,包含非线性惩罚的力储备函数,和归一化扭矩函数。我们的结果表明,与较重的哑铃重量相关的躯干运动明显增加,参与者采用补偿性运动,以保持肩力储备约为最大扭矩的40%。仿真表明,传统和储备功能可以准确预测干线补偿,只有储备功能有效地预测关节扭矩在较重的重量。这些发现表明,战略性地采用补偿性运动来最大程度地减少肩部的努力,并在多个关节上分配负荷以应对无力。我们讨论了力储备成本函数在人体运动最优控制的背景下的含义及其与理解中风后补偿性运动的相关性。
    Following events such as fatigue or stroke, individuals often move their trunks forward during reaching, leveraging a broader muscle group even when only arm movement would suffice. In previous work, we showed the existence of a \"force reserve\": a phenomenon where individuals, when challenged with a heavy weight, adjusted their motor coordination to preserve approximately 40% of their shoulder\'s force. Here, we investigated if such reserve can predict hip, shoulder, and elbow movements and torques resulting from an induced shoulder strength deficit. We engaged 20 healthy participants in a reaching task with incrementally heavier dumbbells, analyzing arm and trunk movements via motion capture and joint torques through inverse dynamics. We simulated these movements using an optimal control model of a 3-degree-of-freedom upper body, contrasting three cost functions: traditional sum of squared torques, a force reserve function incorporating a nonlinear penalty, and a normalized torque function. Our results demonstrate a clear increase in trunk movement correlated with heavier dumbbell weights, with participants employing compensatory movements to maintain a shoulder force reserve of approximately 40% of maximum torque. Simulations showed that while traditional and reserve functions accurately predicted trunk compensation, only the reserve function effectively predicted joint torques under heavier weights. These findings suggest that compensatory movements are strategically employed to minimize shoulder effort and distribute load across multiple joints in response to weakness. We discuss the implications of the force reserve cost function in the context of optimal control of human movements and its relevance for understanding compensatory movements poststroke.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Our study reveals key findings on compensatory movements during upper limb reaching tasks under shoulder strength deficits, as observed poststroke. Using heavy dumbbells with healthy volunteers, we demonstrate how forward trunk displacement conserves around 40% of shoulder strength reserve during reaching. We show that an optimal controller employing a cost function combining squared motor torque and a nonlinear penalty for excessive muscle activation outperforms traditional controllers in predicting torques and compensatory movements in these scenarios.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    精确到达通常需要校正子移动以获得期望的目标。大多数关于到达的研究都集中在单个初始运动上,并暗示皮层编码模型对于所有子运动都是相同的。然而,校正子移动可以显示与初始子移动不同的编码模式,具有跨群体的不同激活模式。两只恒河猴以小目标执行了精确的中心任务。记录来自初级运动皮层中单个单元的神经活动和相关行为数据以评估运动特征。检查了通过寻峰算法识别手速峰值的神经群体数据和单个神经元放电率,以编码初始和校正子运动之间的差异。对单个神经元进行回归模型拟合,其中包括到达向量,position,和速度来预测射击率。对于初始和校正子运动,最大的影响仍然是运动方向。我们观察到,在初始和校正子运动之间,一个很大的子集改变了他们的首选方向超过45度。当调整运动速度时,神经元调制深度也显示出相当大的变化。通过利用主成分分析,初始和校正子运动的神经轨迹通过不同的神经子空间进行。这些发现都表明,皮层内的初始和校正子运动存在不同的神经编码模式。我们假设神经元如何改变编码的这种变化很小,校正子运动可能允许神经空间的较大部分用于编码具有变化幅度和精度水平的更大范围的运动。
    Precision reaching often requires corrective submovements to obtain the desired goal. Most studies of reaching have focused on single initial movements, and implied the cortical encoding model was the same for all submovements. However, corrective submovements may show different encoding patterns from the initial submovement with distinct patterns of activation across the population. Two rhesus macaques performed a precision center-out-task with small targets. Neural activity from single units in the primary motor cortex and associated behavioral data were recorded to evaluate movement characteristics. Neural population data and individual neuronal firing rates identified with a peak finding algorithm to identify peaks in hand speed were examined for encoding differences between initial and corrective submovements. Individual neurons were fitted with a regression model that included the reach vector, position, and speed to predict firing rate. For both initial and corrective submovements, the largest effect remained movement direction. We observed a large subset changed their preferred direction greater than 45° between initial and corrective submovements. Neuronal depth of modulation also showed considerable variation when adjusted for movement speed. By using principal component analysis, neural trajectories of initial and corrective submovements progressed through different neural subspaces. These findings all suggest that different neural encoding patterns exist for initial and corrective submovements within the cortex. We hypothesize that this variation in how neurons change to encode small, corrective submovements might allow for a larger portion of the neural space being used to encode a greater range of movements with varying amplitudes and levels of precision.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Neuronal recordings matched with kinematic behavior were collected in a precision center-out task that often required corrective movements. We reveal large differences in preferred direction and depth of modulation between initial and corrective submovements across the neural population. We then present a model of the neural population describing how these shifts in tuning create different subspaces for signaling initial and corrective movements likely to improve motor precision.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究研究了在向无手臂坐在家中过渡的过程中,婴儿伸手和姿势控制的共同发育。我们观察到,在四个两周的疗程中,患有自闭症的婴儿具有典型的可能性(TL;n=24)和较高的可能性(EL;n=20),整个过渡到无武器坐着(婴儿年龄=4.5-8个月在第一次疗程)。在每届会议上,婴儿坐在有外部支撑或独立的压敏垫上,两个手腕上都戴着磁惯性传感器,伸手去拿中线赠送的玩具。分析的重点是表征和比较到达动作期间的坐姿控制以及在支持坐姿和独立坐姿期间的标准运动学指标。尽管EL婴儿比TL同龄人晚实现无手臂坐姿,在任何测量方面均无组间差异.在整个会议上,在两种情况下,婴儿在同时到达运动期间对坐姿的控制都得到了改善,尽管它们在独立坐着时比在支撑下坐着时更不稳定。在河段的运动学中也有类似的效果,随着时间的推移,总体上有所改善,但有证据表明,相对于支持坐姿,独立控制较差。一起来看,这些发现强调了新兴技能和既定行为之间的相互影响和动态关系。
    This study examined the co-development of infant reaching and postural control across the transition to arms-free sitting at home. We observed infants with typical likelihood (TL; n = 24) and elevated likelihood (EL; n = 20) for autism at four biweekly sessions spanning the transition to arms-free sitting (infant age = 4.5-8 months at first session). At each session, infants sat on a pressure-sensitive mat with external support or independently, wore magneto-inertial sensors on both wrists, and reached for toys presented at midline. Analyses focused on characterizing and comparing control of sitting during reaching actions and standard kinematic metrics of reaching during Supported versus Independent Sitting. Although EL infants achieved arms-free sitting later than TL peers, there were no group differences on any measures. Across sessions, infants\' control of the sitting posture during concurrent reaching movements improved in both contexts, though they were less stable as they reached when sitting independently compared to when sitting with support. A similar effect was apparent in the kinematics of reaches, with overall improvement over time, but evidence of poorer control in Independent relative to Supported Sitting. Taken together, these findings underscore the mutually influential and dynamic relations between emerging skills and well-established behaviors.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    运动声波处理可以改善两个健康受试者的运动控制(例如,学习或完善运动技能)和感觉运动缺陷的人(例如,中风患者和失能个体)。目前尚不清楚改进的电机控制和运动声像学习是否由基于反馈的实时(“在线”)轨迹调整驱动,对多个试验的内部模型进行调整,或者两者兼而有之。我们通过比较运动学和在线达到的误差(即,实时)和终端声音化反馈。我们发现,使用在线反馈进行的达到比使用终端反馈进行的达到明显更不稳定,表明肌肉抽搐增加(即,在线轨迹调整)。使用主题之间的设计,我们发现,在线反馈与改善到达路径和目标的运动学习超过终端反馈有关;然而,使用学科内设计,我们发现,将通过在线发音反馈学习的参与者转换为终末反馈与错误减少相关.因此,我们的结果表明,我们的任务和声音化,运动声波化导致在线轨迹调整,从而改善多次试验的内部模型,但这本身是没有帮助的在线更正。
    Movement sonification can improve motor control in both healthy subjects (e.g., learning or refining a sport skill) and those with sensorimotor deficits (e.g., stroke patients and deafferented individuals). It is not known whether improved motor control and learning from movement sonification are driven by feedback-based real-time (\"online\") trajectory adjustments, adjustments to internal models over multiple trials, or both. We searched for evidence of online trajectory adjustments (muscle twitches) in response to movement sonification feedback by comparing the kinematics and error of reaches made with online (i.e., real-time) and terminal sonification feedback. We found that reaches made with online feedback were significantly more jerky than reaches made with terminal feedback, indicating increased muscle twitching (i.e., online trajectory adjustment). Using a between-subject design, we found that online feedback was associated with improved motor learning of a reach path and target over terminal feedback; however, using a within-subjects design, we found that switching participants who had learned with online sonification feedback to terminal feedback was associated with a decrease in error. Thus, our results suggest that, with our task and sonification, movement sonification leads to online trajectory adjustments which improve internal models over multiple trials, but which themselves are not helpful online corrections.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自适应运动行为取决于分布在大脑中的多个神经系统的协调活动。虽然感觉运动皮层在运动学习中的作用已经得到了很好的确立,高阶大脑系统如何与感觉运动皮层相互作用来指导学习还不太清楚。用功能性核磁共振,我们在基于奖励的运动任务中检查了人脑活动,在该任务中,受试者学会了通过强化反馈来塑造他们的手轨迹。我们将皮质和纹状体功能连接的模式投影到低维流形空间上,并检查了区域在学习过程中如何沿流形扩展和收缩。在早期学习中,我们发现,背侧注意网络中的几个感觉运动区域与显著性/腹侧注意网络区域的协方差增加,与默认模式网络(DMN)区域的协方差降低.在学习后期,这些影响逆转了,感觉运动区域现在与DMN区域的协方差增加。然而,后内侧皮质区域在整个学习阶段表现出相反的模式,它的连通性表明,随着时间的推移,在协调不同网络之间的活动方面发挥了作用。我们的结果建立了支持基于奖励的运动学习的神经变化,并在适应行为时识别了感觉运动与跨模态皮层的功能耦合的不同转变。
    Adaptive motor behavior depends on the coordinated activity of multiple neural systems distributed across the brain. While the role of sensorimotor cortex in motor learning has been well established, how higher-order brain systems interact with sensorimotor cortex to guide learning is less well understood. Using functional MRI, we examined human brain activity during a reward-based motor task where subjects learned to shape their hand trajectories through reinforcement feedback. We projected patterns of cortical and striatal functional connectivity onto a low-dimensional manifold space and examined how regions expanded and contracted along the manifold during learning. During early learning, we found that several sensorimotor areas in the dorsal attention network exhibited increased covariance with areas of the salience/ventral attention network and reduced covariance with areas of the default mode network (DMN). During late learning, these effects reversed, with sensorimotor areas now exhibiting increased covariance with DMN areas. However, areas in posteromedial cortex showed the opposite pattern across learning phases, with its connectivity suggesting a role in coordinating activity across different networks over time. Our results establish the neural changes that support reward-based motor learning and identify distinct transitions in the functional coupling of sensorimotor to transmodal cortex when adapting behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:在上肢的日常生活任务中,在不稳定的姿势情况下,我们必须用手迅速纠正。姿势和到达控制机制涉及上肢任务的准确执行。
    目的:本研究旨在确定在具有非静态目标的达成任务中,不同的姿势稳定性条件对上肢运动性能的影响。
    方法:19名年轻参与者在不同的姿势条件下(两足动物,双足泡沫,和单足泡沫表面)。屏幕上的性能(运动时间和空间误差),平衡(压力位移中心,CoP),在伸手任务期间记录食指运动。
    结果:不稳定性影响手指运动学(位移)和CoP运动学(位移,速度,和平滑度),而不会影响屏幕上的性能(精度和持续时间)。目标更改的时间会影响屏幕上的性能,手指运动学(速度和平滑度),和CoP运动学(位移,速度,和平滑度)。
    结论:姿势和伸手控制系统可以在不太稳定的情况下实现精确的手部动作,甚至在达到具有非静态目标的任务时。姿势和到达控制系统可以在不稳定条件期间保护端部执行器性能,而不是在试验期间以更少的时间来校正运动。
    BACKGROUND: In daily life tasks of the upper limb, we must make quick corrections with our hands in unstable postural situations. Postural and reaching control mechanisms are involved in the accurate execution of upper-limb tasks.
    OBJECTIVE: This research aimed to determine the effect of different postural stability conditions on the motor performance of the upper limb in a reaching task with non-static targets.
    METHODS: 19 young participants performed a reaching task toward targets that exhibited a change in position (at 200 or 600 ms) in different postural conditions (bipedal-firm, bipedal-foam, and unipedal-foam surface). Performance on the screen (motion time and spatial error), balance (center of pressure displacements, CoP), and index finger movements were recorded during the reaching task.
    RESULTS: The instability affects the finger kinematic (displacements) and CoP kinematic (displacements, speed, and smoothness) without affecting the performance on the screen (precision and duration). The timing of target change affects the performance on the screen, finger kinematic (speed and smoothness), and CoP kinematic (displacements, speed, and smoothness).
    CONCLUSIONS: Postural and reaching control systems enable accurate hand motions in less stable situations, even in reaching tasks with non-static targets. The postural and reaching control systems can protect the end-effector performance during unstable conditions but not during trials with less time to correct the motion.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    阿尔茨海默病(AD)的特征是陈述性记忆的初始下降,而非声明性记忆处理保持相对完整。基于错误的运动适应传统上被视为一种非陈述性记忆,但是最近的发现表明,它既涉及快速,声明性和缓慢,非声明性自适应过程。如果陈述性记忆系统与运动适应中的快速过程共享资源,可以假设,但不是慢,AD患者的过程受到干扰。为了测试这个,我们研究了20例早期AD患者和21例年龄匹配的男女对照,采用了依赖于连续暴露于相反力场后的自发恢复的伸展适应模式.使用误差钳测量适应,并表示为适应指数(AI)。尽管AD患者对力场的适应性略低于对照组,两组均表现出自发恢复的效果.AI的时间过程通过分层贝叶斯两状态模型拟合,其中每个动态状态都以保留率和学习率为特征。与对照组相比,快速过程的保留率是AD患者中唯一显着不同(较低)的参数,确认声明者的记忆,快速过程受到AD的干扰。缓慢的适应过程几乎不受影响。由于缓慢的过程只能从错误中微弱地学习,我们的研究结果为痴呆症患者的日常任务无错误学习的临床实践提供了神经计算证据.意义陈述运动适应的实验和建模研究表明,运动记忆形成的双速率过程。我们检查了由于早期阿尔茨海默病而被诊断为陈述性记忆受损的患者的力场运动适应性。使用分层贝叶斯建模,我们展示了他们的速度,但是缓慢的适应过程不会受到影响,这表明运动适应中的快速过程与声明性记忆系统共享资源。
    Alzheimer\'s disease (AD) is characterized by an initial decline in declarative memory, while nondeclarative memory processing remains relatively intact. Error-based motor adaptation is traditionally seen as a form of nondeclarative memory, but recent findings suggest that it involves both fast, declarative, and slow, nondeclarative adaptive processes. If the declarative memory system shares resources with the fast process in motor adaptation, it can be hypothesized that the fast, but not the slow, process is disturbed in AD patients. To test this, we studied 20 early-stage AD patients and 21 age-matched controls of both sexes using a reach adaptation paradigm that relies on spontaneous recovery after sequential exposure to opposing force fields. Adaptation was measured using error clamps and expressed as an adaptation index (AI). Although patients with AD showed slightly lower adaptation to the force field than the controls, both groups demonstrated effects of spontaneous recovery. The time course of the AI was fitted by a hierarchical Bayesian two-state model in which each dynamic state is characterized by a retention and learning rate. Compared to controls, the retention rate of the fast process was the only parameter that was significantly different (lower) in the AD patients, confirming that the memory of the declarative, fast process is disturbed by AD. The slow adaptive process was virtually unaffected. Since the slow process learns only weakly from an error, our results provide neurocomputational evidence for the clinical practice of errorless learning of everyday tasks in people with dementia.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号