Predation risk

捕食风险
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    捕食者对猎物种群施加自上而下的力量,这些影响的强度通常在空间和时间以及人口群体之间有所不同。在有蹄类动物中,捕食风险通常对新生儿后代来说是最大的,一些人认为捕食者可以利用成人活动来定位隐藏的新生儿。然而,迄今为止,很少有实地研究能够直接评估产妇护理对有蹄类新生儿存活的影响。在巨大的土狼捕食压力下使用白尾鹿种群,我们检验了母体离散度假说,这表明母体活动在时间和空间上的分散会在此脆弱的浅表期降低有蹄类新生儿的捕食风险。我们将对这一假设的支持与更常见的关于栖息地条件和内在因素对新生儿生存的影响的假设进行了比较。16周的Fawn生存率为27.7%,土狼占小鹿死亡率的59%。为了支持我们的母体时间扩散假说,我们发现,随着产妇夜间就诊次数的增加(按比例),新生儿存活率下降.小鹿存活的唯一其他显著(p<.1)预测因子是出生时间,随着季节的发展,小鹿的存活率下降。鉴于随着夜间探访比例的增加,小鹿的存活率下降,野猪的存在和人类的干扰会将母鹿和小鹿的活动推向夜间,需要更多的研究来确定管理猪和人类干扰是否可以降低小鹿死亡率。更广泛地说,鉴于招募在有蹄类动物种群动态中的重要性,我们的发现为母性行为如何影响大型动物捕食者-猎物生态学中的捕食风险开辟了一条潜在的重要新的研究路线.
    Predators impose top-down forces on prey populations, with the strength of those effects often varying over space and time and among demographic groups. In ungulates, predation risk is typically greatest for neonatal offspring, with some suggesting that predators can key in on adult activity to locate hidden neonates. However, few field studies to date have been able to directly assess the influence of maternal care on ungulate neonate survival. Using a population of white-tailed deer under heavy coyote predation pressure, we tested the maternal dispersion hypothesis, which suggests the dispersion of maternal activity temporally and spatially attenuates risk of predation for ungulate neonates during this vulnerable altricial phase. We compared support for this hypothesis with more commonly tested hypotheses regarding the influence of habitat conditions and intrinsic factors on neonatal survival. Fawn survival to 16 weeks was 27.7%, with coyotes accounting for 59% of fawn mortalities. In support of our maternal temporal diffusion hypothesis, we found that neonatal survival decreased as more maternal visits (proportionally) occurred at night. The only other significant (p < .1) predictor of fawn survival was birth timing, with fawn survival decreasing as the season progressed. Given that fawn survival declined as the proportion of nighttime visits increased, and that wild pig presence and human disturbance can push doe and fawn activity toward nocturnal hours, additional research is needed to determine whether managing pig and human disturbance can decrease fawn mortality. More broadly, given the importance of recruitment in ungulate population dynamics, our finding opens a potentially important new line of inquiry on how maternal behaviors influence predation risk in large animal predator-prey ecology.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    “恐惧景观”概念为野生动物行为提供了宝贵的见解,然而,它在栖息地管理中的实际整合仍未得到充分探索。在这项研究中,在巴迪亚国家公园的亚热带季风草原进行,尼泊尔,我们的目标是通过多年来弥合这一差距,巴迪亚国家公园的景观尺度实验调查,尼泊尔。该公园在尼泊尔的老虎密度最高(估计密度约为每100平方公里7个人),使我们能够了解栖息地管理对捕食风险和资源可用性的影响,特别是对于三种子宫颈物种:chital(轴),沼泽鹿(Rucervusduvaucelii)和猪鹿(Axisporcinus)。我们使用了不同割草频率的地块(每年0-4次),大小(从小:49平方米到大:3600平方米)和人工施肥类型(无,磷,氮)来评估这些子宫颈物种可能的捕食风险和资源之间的权衡,它们是尼泊尔老虎的主要猎物。我们的结果表明,这些鹿对草地栖息地内感知到的捕食风险有不同的反应。值得注意的是,这些鹿表现出更多使用更大的地块,表示一种感知的安全感,在较大的地块中,颗粒组的发生率较高(3600平方米地块中的平均值=0.1颗粒组m-2与400平方米中的0.07和49平方米中的0.05)。此外,与接受类似处理的较小地块相比,接受割草和施肥处理的较大地块的鹿的使用水平明显更高。特别令人感兴趣的是观察到,在较大的地块内,小鹿和沼泽鹿表现出对中心(核心)区域的更大利用(平均=0.21颗粒组m-2在中心与0.13的边缘)尽管边缘(外围)也为这些鹿提供了有吸引力的资源。相比之下,猪鹿对实验处理没有任何明显的反应,提示对管理干预措施引起的感知捕食风险的潜在物种特异性变化。我们的发现强调了安全感作为管理草地环境中中型鹿栖息地选择的主要决定因素的重要性。这些见解对公园管理者具有实际意义,提供了将“恐惧景观”融入栖息地管理策略的细微差别的理解。这项研究强调,“恐惧景观”概念可以而且应该整合到栖息地管理中,以维持生态系统中微妙的捕食者-猎物动态。
    The \'landscape of fear\' concept offers valuable insights into wildlife behaviour, yet its practical integration into habitat management for conservation remains underexplored. In this study, conducted in the subtropical monsoon grasslands of Bardia National Park, Nepal, we aimed to bridge this gap through a multi-year, landscape-scale experimental investigation in Bardia National Park, Nepal. The park has the highest density of tigers (with an estimated density of ~7 individuals per 100 km2) in Nepal, allowing us to understand the effect of habitat management on predation risk and resource availability especially for three cervid species: chital (Axis axis), swamp deer (Rucervus duvaucelii) and hog deer (Axis porcinus). We used plots with varying mowing frequency (0-4 times per year), size (ranging from small: 49 m2 to large: 3600 m2) and artificial fertilisation type (none, phosphorus, nitrogen) to assess the trade-offs between probable predation risk and resources for these cervid species, which constitute primary prey for tigers in Nepal. Our results showed distinct responses of these deer to perceived predation risk within grassland habitats. Notably, these deer exhibited heightened use of larger plots, indicative of a perceived sense of safety, as evidenced by the higher occurrence of pellet groups in the larger plots (mean = 0.1 pellet groups m-2 in 3600 m2 plots vs. 0.07 in 400 m2 and 0.05 in 49 m2 plots). Furthermore, the level of use by the deer was significantly higher in larger plots that received mowing and fertilisation treatments compared to smaller plots subjected to similar treatments. Of particular interest is the observation that chital and swamp deer exhibited greater utilisation of the centre (core) areas within the larger plots (mean = 0.21 pellet groups m-2 at the centre vs. 0.13 at the edge) despite the edge (periphery) also provided attractive resources to these deer. In contrast, hog deer did not display any discernible reaction to the experimental treatments, suggesting potential species-specific variations in response to perceived predation risk arising from management interventions. Our findings emphasise the importance of a sense of security as a primary determinant of habitat selection for medium-sized deer within managed grassland environments. These insights carry practical implications for park managers, providing a nuanced understanding of integrating the \'landscape of fear\' into habitat management strategies. This study emphasises that the \'landscape of fear\' concept can and should be integrated into habitat management to maintain delicate predator-prey dynamics within ecosystems.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    父母通常在生态环境中遇到可能影响后代发育的压力(跨代可塑性:TGP);然而,父母与物种的互动可能会改变父母对生态压力的反应。在社交缓冲期间,特异性的存在可以减少对应激源的反应或增加从应激源恢复的速度。如果物种可以减轻父母对生态压力的反应,从而减弱压力引起的跨代效应的传递,这可能会对后代产生级联效应。这里,我们同时操纵了父系的社会隔离和在三背刺(Gasterosteusaculeatus)受精之前的捕食风险。我们通过体外受精产生后代,以允许我们分离仅通过精子介导的父系效应(即在没有父系护理的情况下)。如果社会缓冲减轻了父亲暴露于捕食风险引起的TGP,那么,我们预计,与父亲被隔离时相比,当存在特定物种时,捕食暴露的跨代效应会更弱。暴露于捕食者的父亲的后代表现出减少的焦虑样行为,并且倾向于被捕食者更快地捕获。被社会孤立的父亲也有后代,这些后代被活的捕食者更快地捕获,这表明父系社会隔离可能会对后代如何应对生态压力产生适应不良的影响。尽管父系社会隔离和父系捕食风险有累加效应,我们没有发现这些父性治疗之间相互作用的证据,这表明同种异体的存在并不能使父亲和/或后代免受捕食风险的影响.我们的结果表明,社会诱发的压力是一个重要的,然而却被低估了,TGP的中介者,即使在不形成永久社会群体的物种中,也可以引起跨代效应。因此,未来的研究应考虑父母的社会环境如何影响对生态压力源的内部和跨代反应。
    Parents routinely encounter stress in the ecological environment that can affect offspring development (transgenerational plasticity: TGP); however, parents\' interactions with conspecifics may alter how parents respond to ecological stressors. During social buffering, the presence of conspecifics can reduce the response to or increase the speed of recovery from a stressor. This may have cascading effects on offspring if conspecifics can mitigate parental responses to ecological stress in ways that blunt the transmission of stress-induced transgenerational effects. Here, we simultaneously manipulated both paternal social isolation and experience with predation risk prior to fertilisation in threespined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). We generated offspring via in-vitro fertilisation to allow us to isolate paternal effects mediated via sperm alone (i.e. in the absence of paternal care). If social buffering mitigates TGP induced by paternal exposure to predation risk, then we expect the transgenerational effects of predation exposure to be weaker when a conspecific is present compared to when the father is isolated. Offspring of predator-exposed fathers showed reduced anxiety-like behaviour and tended to be captured faster by the predator. Fathers who were socially isolated also had offspring that were captured faster by a live predator, suggesting that paternal social isolation may have maladaptive effects on how offspring respond to ecological stressors. Despite additive effects of paternal social isolation and paternal predation risk, we found no evidence of an interaction between these paternal treatments, suggesting that the presence of a conspecific did not buffer fathers and/or offspring from the effects of predation risk. Our results suggest that socially induced stress is an important, yet underappreciated, mediator of TGP and can elicit transgenerational effects even in species that do not form permanent social groups. Future studies should therefore consider how the parental social environment can affect both within and trans-generational responses to ecological stressors.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多树种采用快速种子发芽来逃避啮齿动物的捕食风险。部分消耗的种子上的物理种子损伤和啮齿动物的唾液可能作为种子加速发芽过程的线索。然而,这些因素对种子发芽率和速度的影响尚不清楚。在这项研究中,我们调查了这种影响对四种树种(栓皮工程,Q.serrata,问Q.glauca)在被四种啮齿动物部分食用后(Leopoldamysedwards,Niviventer饱食者,N.孔子,Apodemusdraco),通过一系列的实验。我们还通过分析啮齿动物受损种子的发芽率和时间与性状之间的关系,研究了种子性状如何影响啮齿动物的种子受损程度。我们发现,人工和啮齿动物受损的种子表现出明显更高的种子发芽率和速度,与完整的种子相比。此外,种子上的啮齿动物唾液对种子的发芽率和发芽速度没有显着影响。此外,我们观察到几个种子性状(包括种子质量,涂层厚度,和蛋白质含量)以及种子发芽率和速度。这些相关性可能是由于它们的有益性状对抗啮齿动物的种子损害。总的来说,我们的结果强调了啮齿动物对种子的物理损伤(而不是它们的唾液)在促进树种种子发芽方面的重要作用,以及啮齿动物和树木之间潜在的共生关系。此外,我们的结果可能对森林恢复有一些影响,人类或无人机故意播种或分散轻微受损的种子可能会增加成功种子再生的可能性。
    Many tree species adopt fast seed germination to escape the predation risk by rodents. Physical seed damage and the saliva of rodents on partially consumed seeds may act as cues for seeds to accelerate germination process. However, the impacts of these factors on seed germination rate and speed remain unclear. In this study, we investigated such impacts on the germination rate and speed (reversal of germination time) of four tree species (Quercus variabilis, Q. serrata, Q. acutissima, Q. glauca) after partial consumption by four rodent species (Leopoldamys edwards, Niviventer fulvescens, N. confucianus, Apodemus draco), through a series of experiments. We also examined how seed traits may affect the seed damage degree by rodents by analyzing the relationship between the germination rate and time of rodent-damaged seed and the traits. We found that, artificially- and rodent-damaged seeds exhibited a significantly higher seed germination rate and speed, compared to intact seeds. Also, the rodent saliva on seeds showed no significant effect on seed germination rate and speed. Furthermore, we observed significant positive correlations between several seed traits (including the seed mass, coat thickness, and protein content) and the seed germination rate and speed. These correlations are likely due to their beneficial traits countering seed damage by rodents. Overall, our results highlight the significant role of physical seed damage by rodents (rather than their saliva) in facilitating seed germination of tree species, and potential mutualism between rodents and trees. Additionally, our results may have some implications in forest restoration, such that intentionally sowing or dispersing slightly damaged seeds by humans or drones may increase the likelihood of successful seed regeneration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    父母的照料受多种内分泌机制的调节。在这些荷尔蒙中,催乳素(PRL)参与父母行为的表达。尽管人们一致认为PRL可以调节父母努力与年龄和身体状况的变化,它在调整父母的努力以适应波动的环境条件中的作用,包括改变捕食压力,仍在等待进一步调查。为了揭示这种知识差距,我们依赖于对雌性常见的eff的长期监测(n=1277次繁殖尝试,2012-2022)在波动的捕食风险下进行孵化,以调查基线PRL水平与女性最低年龄之间的联系,身体状况,离合器尺寸,环境参数(捕食压力,气候,巢微生境)和孵化成功。我们预测老年女性的PRL会更高,那些状态更好或孵化更大的离合器。我们还预测,在具有挑战性的环境条件(高捕食压力或恶劣的气候条件)下筑巢时,雌性会减少父母的努力,转化为降低基线PRL水平。我们还探讨了PRL水平的变化,雌性特征和环境参数与孵化成功有关。根据我们的预测,PRL水平与身体状况和女性年龄呈正相关(在年龄最大的育种者出现衰老下降之前)。然而,我们没有观察到PRL水平因捕食压力升高而降低人群水平或个体水平.在研究期间,人口水平的基线PRL水平反而增加了,随着捕食威胁的上升,而且还会增加女性的身体状况和年龄。虽然我们没有提供基线PRL水平和捕食风险之间直接关联的证据,我们的结果支持这样的观点,即升高的基线PRL水平在内部约束下促进孵化成功(在年轻时,缺乏经验,育种者或孵化大型离合器的人)或限制环境条件(在高捕食压力或恶劣的气候和觅食条件下)。最后,基线PRL水平的低重复性和高年际变异性突出了基线PRL水平的个体内部相当大的灵活性.进一步的研究应该探索父母努力改变环境条件的灵活性,关注基线和应激诱导的PRL水平。
    Parental care is regulated by multiple endocrine mechanisms. Among these hormones, prolactin (PRL) is involved in the expression of parental behaviors. Despite the consensus that PRL mediates variation in parental effort with age and body condition, its role in the adjustment of parental effort to fluctuating environmental conditions, including changing predation pressure, still awaits further investigation. To shed light on this knowledge gap, we relied on a long-term monitoring of female common eiders Somateria mollissima (n = 1277 breeding attempts, 2012-2022) incubating under fluctuating predation risk to investigate the link between baseline PRL levels and female minimum age, body condition, clutch size, environmental parameters (predation pressure, climate, nest microhabitat) and hatching success. We predicted that PRL would be higher in older females, those in better condition or incubating larger clutches. We also predicted that females would reduce parental effort when nesting under challenging environmental conditions (high predation pressure or poor climatic conditions), translated into reduced baseline PRL levels. We also explored how variation in PRL levels, female characteristics and environmental parameters were related to hatching success. Following our predictions, PRL levels were positively associated with body condition and female age (before showing a senescent decline in the oldest breeders). However, we did not observe any population-level or individual-level reduction in PRL levels in response to increasing predation pressure. Population-level baseline PRL levels instead increased over the study period, coincident with rising predation threat, but also increasing female body condition and age. While we did not provide evidence for a direct association between baseline PRL levels and predation risk, our results support the idea that elevated baseline PRL levels promote hatching success under internal constraints (in young, inexperienced, breeders or those incubating a large clutch) or constraining environmental conditions (during years of high predation pressure or poor climatic and foraging conditions). Finally, the low repeatability of baseline PRL levels and high interannual variability highlight considerable within-individual flexibility in baseline PRL levels. Further research should explore flexibility in parental effort to changing environmental conditions, focusing on both baseline and stress-induced PRL levels.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    警惕是一种常见的行为适应,可以在逃脱为时已晚之前增加发现捕食者的机会。从长远来看,行为特征在个体之间通常是可重复的,暗示个性上的差异。早期的研究已经记录了分配给警惕的时间的个体一致性。然而,关于个体一致性从一个时刻到另一个时刻实现警惕的方式,以及个体之间不同的警惕模式是否与生存相关,人们知之甚少。我们的目的是确定合作饲养员的哨兵在警惕性上是否表现出个体一致性,以及个体变异是否与年生存率有关。在有利位置的哨兵比赛中,佛罗里达磨砂鸟(Aphelocomacorenulescens)将头从一侧转到另一侧以监视周围环境。在三个野外季节,我们发现,转头频率在育种者中可重复,但在少年或非育种助手中不可重复。育种者的中等可重复性与生存率无关。我们的结果表明,佛罗里达磨砂jay哨兵的转头频率在育种者中是可重复的,但在经验较少的少年或助手中却不是,因此,随着个体年龄的增长,可能变得更加可重复。在我们的研究中,个体警惕性变异与生存有关的假设没有得到支持,需要进一步研究。
    Vigilance is a common behavioural adaptation to increase the chances of detecting predators before it is too late to escape. Behavioural traits are often repeatable among individuals over the long term, suggesting differences in personality. Earlier studies have documented individual consistency in the time allocated to vigilance. However, little is known about individual consistency in the ways vigilance is achieved from one moment to another and whether different patterns of vigilance among individuals are associated with survival. We aimed to determine whether sentinels of a cooperative breeder showed individual consistency in their vigilance and if individual variation was related to annual survival. During sentinel bouts from vantage points, Florida scrub-jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) turn their heads from side to side to monitor their surroundings. Over three field seasons, we found that the head-turning frequency was repeatable in breeders but not in juveniles or non-breeding helpers. The moderate repeatability in breeders was not related to survival. Our results suggest that the head-turning frequency in sentinels of the Florida scrub-jay is repeatable in breeders but not in less experienced juveniles or helpers and, therefore, likely becomes more repeatable as individuals age. The assumption that individual variation in vigilance is related to survival was unsupported in our study and requires further study.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    捕食威胁是许多猎物行为的主要驱动因素。动物可以根据环境中的线索识别它们的相对捕食风险,包括捕食者或猎物释放的视觉和/或化学线索。当捕食威胁很高时,猎物通常通过改变其行为来降低其检测和/或捕获的概率。这里,我们测试克隆鱼,亚马逊莫利(Poeciliaformosa),对捕食线索的行为反应。我们在“风险”下测量了攻击性和社交行为,存在来自掠食性鱼类和受伤物种的化学线索,和控制上下文(不存在风险线索)。我们预测,由于侵略可能会增加它们对捕食者的可见度,而浅滩则会通过稀释效应减少捕获它们的机会,因此在风险环境下,对模拟入侵者的侵略将减少其社交能力。正如预测的那样,我们发现,当存在风险线索时,亚马逊莫利斯花了更多的时间与特定对象在一起,然而,他们并没有减少他们的侵略。这突出了许多小型浅滩物种表现出的“数量安全”行为反应的一般结果,包括这些克隆鱼,这表明,与通过减少积极的特异性相互作用来限制其可检测性相比,莫利斯可能将这种反应视为更有效的抗捕食者反应。
    Predation threat is a major driver of behavior in many prey species. Animals can recognize their relative risk of predation based on cues in the environment, including visual and/or chemical cues released by a predator or from its prey. When threat of predation is high, prey often respond by altering their behavior to reduce their probability of detection and/or capture. Here, we test how a clonal fish, the Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa), behaviorally responds to predation cues. We measured aggressive and social behaviors both under \'risk\', where chemical cues from predatory fish and injured conspecifics were present, and control contexts (no risk cues present). We predicted that mollies would exhibit reduced aggression towards a simulated intruder and increased sociability under risk contexts as aggression might increase their visibility to a predator and shoaling should decrease their chance of capture through the dilution effect. As predicted, we found that Amazon mollies spent more time with a conspecific when risk cues were present, however they did not reduce their aggression. This highlights the general result of the \'safety in numbers\' behavioral response that many small shoaling species exhibit, including these clonal fish, which suggests that mollies may view this response as a more effective anti-predator response compared to limiting their detectability by reducing aggressive conspecific interactions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多新热带甲虫呈现出模仿红眼苍蝇的着色模式,大概是回避的模仿模型。然而,捕食者在自然界中选择规避模仿物的作用尚未得到检验。在现场实验中,我们使用了一种专门模拟苍蝇的甲虫物种的无毒橡皮泥复制品,我们把它放在甲虫的寄主植物上。我们表明,涂有红色斑点的复制品模拟肉蝇的眼睛,其捕食率远低于对照复制品。我们发现喙是攻击橡皮泥复制品的最常见迹象,强调鸟类施加的潜在选择压力。与甲虫大小匹配的复制品比更小或更大的复制品遭受更高的捕食。在温暖和潮湿季节暴露的甲虫复制品的捕食率也较高,当成年甲虫出现时。我们的结果支持捕食者介导的模拟甲虫选择,强调类似苍蝇眼睛的红色斑点是减少鸟类捕食者攻击的重要特征。
    AbstractMany Neotropical beetles present coloration patterns mimicking red-eyed flies, which are presumably evasive mimicry models. However, the role of predators in selecting for evasive mimics in nature remains untested. In a field experiment, we used nontoxic plasticine replicas of a specialized fly-mimicking beetle species, which we placed on the host plants of the beetles. We show that replicas painted with reddish patches simulating the eyes of flesh flies experienced a much lower predation rate than control replicas. We found that beak marks were the most frequent signs of attack on plasticine replicas, underlining the potential selective pressure exerted by birds. Replicas that matched the size of the beetles suffered higher predation than smaller or larger replicas. The predation rate was also higher for beetle replicas exposed during the warm and wet season, when adult beetles occur. Our results support predator-mediated selection of mimic beetles, highlighting that reddish spots resembling flies\' eyes comprise an important trait in reducing attack by avian predators.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    捕食者的非消耗性效应(NCE)可以改变猎物的觅食时间和栖息地的使用,有可能降低健身。猎物可以通过提高警惕来缓解NCE,咀嚼-警惕同步,和天敌的时空回避。我们量化了墨西哥狼(Canislupusbaileyi)捕食风险与麋鹿(Cervuscanadensis)行为之间的关系。我们对成年雌性麋鹿进行了行为观察,并使用墨西哥狼的GPS项圈数据制定了捕食风险指数,麋鹿被狼杀死的地点,和景观协变量。我们比较了先验模型,以确定成年女性行为和多任务处理的最佳预测因素。量化空间和时间捕食风险的指标最具预测性。警惕与捕食风险增加呈正相关。捕食风险对觅食和休息的影响在昼夜期间有所不同。在中午,狼的活动较低,在高风险地区,觅食的可能性增加,而休息的可能性降低。在麋鹿和狼最活跃的黄昏时期,捕食风险增加与警惕性提高和觅食略有减少相关.我们的结果表明,麋鹿通过交易休息觅食暂时避免了墨西哥狼的捕食风险,在行为研究中经常没有评估的权衡。多任务处理的可能性取决于冠层的开放性以及产妇期与捕食风险之间的相互作用;在高危地区,分娩前多任务处理减少,分娩后增加。开放性与多任务处理成反比。这些结果表明,成年雌性麋鹿正在根据资源的可用性/质量改变所使用的警惕类型,当前的能量需求,和捕食风险。我们的结果强调了潜在的重要性,但是经常被排除的行为和权衡的猎物物种可能会用来减少捕食的间接影响,并为我们对捕食者-猎物动力学的理解提供额外的背景。
    Predator non-consumptive effects (NCE) can alter prey foraging time and habitat use, potentially reducing fitness. Prey can mitigate NCEs by increasing vigilance, chewing-vigilance synchronization, and spatiotemporal avoidance of predators. We quantified the relationship between Mexican wolf (Canis lupus baileyi) predation risk and elk (Cervus canadensis) behavior. We conducted behavioral observations on adult female elk and developed predation risk indices using GPS collar data from Mexican wolves, locations of elk killed by wolves, and landscape covariates. We compared a priori models to determine the best predictors of adult female behavior and multitasking. Metrics that quantified both spatial and temporal predation risk were the most predictive. Vigilance was positively associated with increased predation risk. The effect of predation risk on foraging and resting differed across diurnal periods. During midday when wolf activity was lower, the probability of foraging increased while resting decreased in high-risk areas. During crepuscular periods when elk and wolves were most active, increased predation risk was associated with increased vigilance and slight decreases in foraging. Our results suggest elk are temporally avoiding predation risk from Mexican wolves by trading resting for foraging, a trade-off often not evaluated in behavioral studies. Probability of multitasking depended on canopy openness and an interaction between maternal period and predation risk; multitasking decreased prior to parturition and increased post parturition in high-risk areas. Openness was inversely related to multitasking. These results suggest adult female elk are altering the type of vigilance used depending on resource availability/quality, current energetic needs, and predation risk. Our results highlight potentially important, but often-excluded behaviors and trade-offs prey species may use to reduce the indirect effects of predation and contribute additional context to our understanding of predator-prey dynamics.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    水生猎物具有令人印象深刻的能力,可以从各种化学线索中提取信息。例如,他们可以使用受伤人员在捕食者袭击期间释放的警报提示来了解捕食风险,他们还可以在互动中区分亲属和非亲属。然而,尚不清楚动物是否可以将有关捕食风险的信息与受到威胁的特定个体的亲属识别相结合。为了检查警报提示中个体的相关性如何影响行为,我们使用了自受精的雌雄同体红树林(Kryptoolebiasmarmoratus),其中谱系通过自交产生遗传相同的后代。我们在两个异交水平不同的种群中进行了探索。我们测量了暴露于警报提示之前和之后的活动,警报提示是由来自他们自己的谱系或来自同一人群的无关谱系的个体(成人或胚胎)产生的。鱼对胚胎警报线索反应微弱,但是当警报提示来自不相关的谱系时,与来自自己谱系的警报提示相比,它们的活动更倾向于减少,特别是在来自异类种群的鱼类中。相比之下,提示相关性对成人警报提示的反应没有影响,但有强烈的群体效应。具体来说,与主要自交人群的个体相比,异交人群的个体对警报提示的反应更强烈。我们讨论了主要组织相容性复合体在线索检测中的潜在作用,成人与胚胎警报线索在浓度和信息方面的差异,以及种群和遗传谱系在产生和检测化学线索方面的潜在差异。这种亲属识别是否提供了适应性益处,或者仅仅是能够检测到活着的个体的亲密关系的结果,将是未来研究的一个令人兴奋的领域。
    Aquatic prey have impressive abilities to extract information from a variety of chemical cues. For example, they can use the alarm cues released by wounded individuals during a predator attack to learn about predation risk, and they can also distinguish kin from non-kin individuals during interactions. However, it remains unclear whether animals can combine this information on predation risk with kin recognition of the particular individuals under threat. To examine how the relatedness of the individuals in alarm cue affects behaviour we used the self-fertilizing hermaphroditic mangrove rivulus (Kryptolebias marmoratus), in which lineages produce genetically identical offspring through selfing. We explored this in two populations that differ in their level of outcrossing. We measured activity before and after exposure to alarm cue made from individuals (either adults or embryos) from their own lineage or an unrelated lineage from the same population. Fish responded weakly to embryo alarm cues, but tended to reduce their activity more when the alarm cues were from an unrelated lineage compared to alarm cues from their own lineage, particularly in fish from the outcrossing population. In contrast, there was no effect of cue relatedness on the response to adult alarm cues but there was a strong population effect. Specifically, individuals from the outcrossing population tended to react more strongly to alarm cues compared to individuals from the predominantly selfing population. We discuss the potential roles of the major histocompatibility complex in cue detection, differences between adult vs embryo alarm cues in terms of concentration and information, and underlying differences among populations and genetic lineages in their production and detection of chemical cues. Whether this kin recognition offers adaptive benefits or is simply a consequence of being able to detect relatedness in living individuals would be an exciting area for future research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号