Pongo

Pongo
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    游戏被认为在个体发育的不同时间服务于不同的目的。游戏的性质和频率预计会随着发展轨迹和社会生态环境而相应变化。猩猩提供了一个机会,可以通过其扩展的不成熟阶段来解开孤独和社交游戏的本体遗传轨迹,以及种群和物种之间的社会生态变化。我们评估了两个种群(Pongopygmaeuswurmbii,在团南,婆罗洲,还有Suaq的P.Abelii,Sumatra),年龄(0-11岁),性别,和社会背景,使用超过11500小时的全天局灶性观测数据。我们发现了不同游戏类型的独立年龄轨迹,在社交游戏之前,孤独的物体和孤独的运动达到顶峰。社交游戏伙伴在个体发育过程中改变了,未成熟的男性比女性更有可能参与非母亲的社交活动。总的来说,在Suaq的社交游戏比Tuanan更频繁,链接到更频繁的合作伙伴可用性。此外,每次与特定物种相关,Tuanan不成熟的人与Suaq的同龄人一样参与社交游戏,暗示类似的内在动机。在团南社交性较低的人群中,水果的可获得性增加与更长的协会和社交游戏频率增加相关,但不是在Suaq.我们对猩猩的发现支持来自其他物种的证据,即不同的游戏类型遵循不同的发育轨迹,随性别而变化,社会机会,和生态背景。尽管绘制功能推论具有挑战性,反映成人社交能力和行为方式的不同发展轨迹可能表明游戏有几个,个体发育过程中的非互斥功能。
    在线版本包含补充材料,可在10.1007/s10764-023-00414-2获得。
    Play is thought to serve different purposes at different times during ontogeny. The nature and frequency of play are expected to change accordingly over the developmental trajectory and with socio-ecological context. Orangutans offer the opportunity to disentangle the ontogenetic trajectories of solitary and social play with their extended immature phase, and socio-ecological variation among populations and species. We evaluated the frequency of play in 39 immature individuals across two populations (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii, at Tuanan, Borneo, and P. abelii at Suaq, Sumatra), age (0-11 years), sex, and social context, using more than 11 500 h of full-day focal observation data. We found independent age trajectories of different play types, with solitary object and solitary locomotor peaking before social play. Social play partners changed during ontogeny, and male immatures were more likely to engage in non-mother social play than females. Overall, social play was more frequent at Suaq than Tuanan, linked to the more frequent availability of partners. Furthermore, per time in association with conspecifics, Tuanan immatures were as likely to engage in social play as their peers at Suaq, suggesting similar intrinsic motivation. Increasing fruit availability correlated with both longer associations and increased social play frequency in the less sociable population of Tuanan, but not at Suaq. Our findings on orangutans support evidence from other species that different play types follow different developmental trajectories, vary with sex, social opportunities, and ecological context. Although drawing functional inferences is challenging, the distinct developmental trajectories reflecting adult sociability and behavioral repertoires may indicate that play serves several, non-mutually exclusive functions during ontogeny.
    UNASSIGNED: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10764-023-00414-2.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当被提供直接利益或更大利益的选择时,后来的奖励,我们可能会通过选择即时满足而表现得不耐烦。尽管如此,当我们可以提前做出同样的决定并为未来做计划时,我们倾向于做出更有耐心的选择。这里,我们探索了类人猿是否具有人类决策的核心特征,通常被称为动态不一致。我们发现猩猩,黑猩猩,大猩猩在选择即时奖励和较大的后期奖励时,往往表现得不耐烦,个体之间存在相当大的差异,这是现场常用的测试方法。然而,包括两种选择的前端延迟,他们的决定变得更加耐心和同质。这些结果表明,大猿是动态不一致的。他们还建议,当在未来的结果之间做出选择时,他们比以前报道的更有耐心。我们主张在比较研究中纳入不同的时间范围,特别是考虑到跨期选择和面向未来的行为的交织。
    When presented with the option of either an immediate benefit or a larger, later reward, we may behave impatiently by choosing instant gratification. Nonetheless, when we can make the same decision ahead of time and plan for the future, we tend to make more patient choices. Here, we explored whether great apes share this core feature of human decision-making, often referred to as dynamic inconsistency. We found that orangutans, bonobos, and gorillas tended to act impatiently and with considerable variability between individuals when choosing between an immediate reward and a larger-later reward, which is a commonly employed testing method in the field. However, with the inclusion of a front-end delay for both alternatives, their decisions became more patient and homogeneous. These results show that great apes are dynamically inconsistent. They also suggest that, when choosing between future outcomes, they are more patient than previously reported. We advocate for the inclusion of diverse time ranges in comparative research, especially considering the intertwinement of intertemporal choices and future-oriented behavior.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    从猩猩13号染色体的最新基因组组装NHGRI_mPonAbe1-v2.0_NCBI(GCF_028885655.2)中,我们使用新型高精度GRM2023算法和全局重复图(GRM)计算了精确的α卫星高阶重复(HOR)结构和单体距离(MD)图。这项研究严格鉴定了猩猩13号染色体着丝粒中的α卫星HOR,发现了一种新型的59merHOR,这是迄今为止在任何灵长类动物中鉴定出的最长的HOR单位。此外,它揭示了三个HOR的第一个相互交织的序列,18mer/27mer/45merHORS,在所有HOR副本中具有共同对齐的“主干”。主要的7merHOR展示了Willard类型的规范副本,尽管阵列的某些部分显示出明显的不规则性。相比之下,14merHOR形成一个常规的Willard类型的HOR数组。令人惊讶的是,GRM2023对人类基因组组装T2T-CHM13v2.0的13号染色体的高精度分析揭示了仅存在7merHOR,尽管猩猩和人类基因组组装都来自全基因组猎枪序列。
    From the recent genome assembly NHGRI_mPonAbe1-v2.0_NCBI (GCF_028885655.2) of orangutan chromosome 13, we computed the precise alpha satellite higher-order repeat (HOR) structure using the novel high-precision GRM2023 algorithm with Global Repeat Map (GRM) and Monomer Distance (MD) diagrams. This study rigorously identified alpha satellite HORs in the centromere of orangutan chromosome 13, discovering a novel 59mer HOR-the longest HOR unit identified in any primate to date. Additionally, it revealed the first intertwined sequence of three HORs, 18mer/27mer/45mer HORs, with a common aligned \"backbone\" across all HOR copies. The major 7mer HOR exhibits a Willard\'s-type canonical copy, although some segments of the array display significant irregularities. In contrast, the 14mer HOR forms a regular Willard\'s-type HOR array. Surprisingly, the GRM2023 high-precision analysis of chromosome 13 of human genome assembly T2T-CHM13v2.0 reveals the presence of only a 7mer HOR, despite both the orangutan and human genome assemblies being derived from whole genome shotgun sequences.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们确定了五个不同的全长人盐皮质激素受体(MR)基因,包含984个氨基酸(MR-984)或988个氨基酸(MR-988)。可以通过Lys的存在或不存在来区分,Cys,Ser,和Trp(KCSW)在它们的DNA结合结构域(DBD)中以及在它们的氨基末端结构域(NTD)中的密码子180和241处的突变。两个人MR-KCSW基因在其NTD中包含(Val-180,Val-241)或(Ile-180,Val-241),和三个人类MR-984基因包含(Ile-180,Ala-241),(Val-180,Val-241),或(Ile-180,Val-241)。尚未克隆具有(Ile-180,Ala-241)的人MR-KCSW。相比之下,黑猩猩包含四个MR:两个MR-988,其DBD中有KCSW,或两台DBD中没有KCSW的MR-984。黑猩猩MR在其NTD中仅包含(Ile180,Val-241)。尚未克隆在NTD中具有(Val-180,Val-241)或(Ile-180,Ala-241)的黑猩猩MR。大猩猩和猩猩各自包含一个在DBD中带有KCSW的MR-988和一个没有KCSW的MR-984,并且这些MR在其NTD中仅包含(Ile-180,Val-241)。尚未克隆NTD中具有(Val-180,Val-241)或(Ile-180,Ala-241)的大猩猩MR或猩猩MR。一起,这些数据表明,在NTD中具有(Val-180,Val-241)或(Ile-180,Ala-241)的人类MR是在人类和黑猩猩从其共同祖先分化后进化而来的。考虑到肾脏MR在人类发育中的多种功能,大脑,心,皮肤,和肺,以及与糖皮质激素受体相互作用的MR活性,我们认为,黑猩猩中不存在的人类MR的进化可能对人类从黑猩猩的进化很重要。人体MR介导的皮质类固醇生理反应的研究,黑猩猩,大猩猩,猩猩和猩猩可以提供对人类及其近亲进化的见解。
    We identified five distinct full-length human mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) genes containing either 984 amino acids (MR-984) or 988 amino acids (MR-988), which can be distinguished by the presence or absence of Lys, Cys, Ser, and Trp (KCSW) in their DNA-binding domain (DBD) and mutations at codons 180 and 241 in their amino-terminal domain (NTD). Two human MR-KCSW genes contain either (Val-180, Val-241) or (Ile-180, Val-241) in their NTD, and three human MR-984 genes contain either (Ile-180, Ala-241), (Val-180, Val-241), or (Ile-180, Val-241). Human MR-KCSW with (Ile-180, Ala-241) has not been cloned. In contrast, chimpanzees contain four MRs: two MR-988s with KCSW in their DBD, or two MR-984s without KCSW in their DBD. Chimpanzee MRs only contain (Ile180, Val-241) in their NTD. A chimpanzee MR with either (Val-180, Val-241) or (Ile-180, Ala-241) in the NTD has not been cloned. Gorillas and orangutans each contain one MR-988 with KCSW in the DBD and one MR-984 without KCSW, and these MRs only contain (Ile-180, Val-241) in their NTD. A gorilla MR or orangutan MR with either (Val-180, Val-241) or (Ile-180, Ala-241) in the NTD has not been cloned. Together, these data suggest that human MRs with (Val-180, Val-241) or (Ile-180, Ala-241) in the NTD evolved after humans and chimpanzees diverged from their common ancestor. Considering the multiple functions in human development of the MR in kidney, brain, heart, skin, and lungs, as well as MR activity in interaction with the glucocorticoid receptor, we suggest that the evolution of human MRs that are absent in chimpanzees may have been important in the evolution of humans from chimpanzees. Investigation of the physiological responses to corticosteroids mediated by the MR in humans, chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans may provide insights into the evolution of humans and their closest relatives.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: News
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类着丝粒由于其重复性质和大尺寸1而传统上非常难以测序和组装。因此,人类着丝粒变异的模式及其进化和功能的模型仍然不完整,尽管中心粒是突变最快的区域之一2,3。这里,使用长读数测序,我们对第二个人类基因组的所有着丝粒进行了完全测序和组装,并将其与完成的参考基因组4,5进行了比较。我们发现,两组着丝粒与其独特的侧翼相比,单核苷酸变异至少增加了4.1倍,并且大小变化高达3倍。此外,我们发现,由于出现了新的α-卫星高阶重复序列(HOR),因此无法使用标准方法可靠地对齐45.8%的着丝粒序列。DNA甲基化和CENP-A染色质免疫沉淀实验表明,26%的着丝粒的动粒位置差异>500kb。为了理解进化的变化,我们选择了6条染色体,并从普通黑猩猩中测序和组装了31个直系同源着丝粒,猩猩和猕猴基因组。比较分析显示,α卫星HOR几乎完全周转,每个物种的α卫星HOR具有特征性的特殊变化。人类单倍型的系统发育重建支持限制在整个中心粒的短(p)和长(q)臂之间没有重组,并揭示了新的α卫星HOR共享单系起源,提供了一种估计人类着丝粒DNA的盐分扩增和突变率的策略。
    Human centromeres have been traditionally very difficult to sequence and assemble owing to their repetitive nature and large size1. As a result, patterns of human centromeric variation and models for their evolution and function remain incomplete, despite centromeres being among the most rapidly mutating regions2,3. Here, using long-read sequencing, we completely sequenced and assembled all centromeres from a second human genome and compared it to the finished reference genome4,5. We find that the two sets of centromeres show at least a 4.1-fold increase in single-nucleotide variation when compared with their unique flanks and vary up to 3-fold in size. Moreover, we find that 45.8% of centromeric sequence cannot be reliably aligned using standard methods owing to the emergence of new α-satellite higher-order repeats (HORs). DNA methylation and CENP-A chromatin immunoprecipitation experiments show that 26% of the centromeres differ in their kinetochore position by >500 kb. To understand evolutionary change, we selected six chromosomes and sequenced and assembled 31 orthologous centromeres from the common chimpanzee, orangutan and macaque genomes. Comparative analyses reveal a nearly complete turnover of α-satellite HORs, with characteristic idiosyncratic changes in α-satellite HORs for each species. Phylogenetic reconstruction of human haplotypes supports limited to no recombination between the short (p) and long (q) arms across centromeres and reveals that novel α-satellite HORs share a monophyletic origin, providing a strategy to estimate the rate of saltatory amplification and mutation of human centromeric DNA.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们使用病原体基因组学来测试波恩一家博物馆的猩猩标本,德国,确定动物的起源和死亡的情况。我们在样本中发现了猴痘病毒基因组,并确定它们代表了1965年鹿特丹鹿特丹动物园爆发的病例,荷兰。
    We used pathogen genomics to test orangutan specimens from a museum in Bonn, Germany, to identify the origin of the animals and the circumstances of their death. We found monkeypox virus genomes in the samples and determined that they represent cases from a 1965 outbreak at Rotterdam Zoo in Rotterdam, the Netherlands.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类和其他几种动物已经证明了使用熟悉度来识别他们以前看过图像的能力。在之前的实验中,猩猩未能在内存任务中表现出熟悉度,即使没有其他解决方案。我们测试了习惯的证据,对重复刺激的反应降低,作为一个行为指标,重复的图像是熟悉的对象。猴子和猩猩在四个目标中选择了最小的目标,而计算机化的图像则被视为干扰物。在干扰物图像熟悉的情况下,比较了完成目标查找任务的延迟,重复图像,一本小说,从未见过的图像,或者没有分心者在场。恒河猴表现出明显的习惯性,比猩猩更习惯,在四个实验中的每一个。猩猩仅在四个实验之一中显示出统计上可靠的习惯。这些结果与先前的研究结果一致,在先前的研究中,猩猩未能表现出熟悉程度。因为我们期望熟悉和习惯是进化上古老的记忆过程,我们努力解释这些令人惊讶的,但一致的发现。需要进一步的研究来确定为什么猩猩以这种特殊的方式对计算机图像做出反应。
    Humans and several other species of animals have demonstrated the ability to use familiarity to recognize that they have seen images before. In prior experiments, orangutans failed to show use of familiarity in memory tasks, even when other solutions were not available. We tested for evidence of habituation, a decreased response to repeated stimuli, as a behavioral indicator that repeated images were familiar to subjects. Monkeys and orangutans selected the smallest target out of four while computerized images were presented as distractors. Latency to complete the target-finding task was compared between conditions in which the distractor image was a familiar, repeating image, a novel, never-before-seen image, or no distractor was present. Rhesus macaques showed significant habituation, and significantly more habituation than orangutans, in each of four experiments. Orangutans showed statistically reliable habituation in only one of the four experiments. These results are consistent with previous research in which orangutans failed to demonstrate familiarity. Because we expect that familiarity and habituation are evolutionarily ancient memory processes, we struggle to explain these surprising, but consistent findings. Future research is needed to determine why orangutans respond to computerized images in this peculiar way.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Pongo化石的稀有性与中更新世的精确绝对年代有关,这阻碍了我们对中国南方第四纪猩猩的分类学和时空分布的理解。这里,我们报告了布冰盆地中山洞新发现的113颗孤立牙齿化石的样本,广西,中国南方。我们描述了中山洞的Pongo标本,并将其与化石Pongo物种的其他样本进行了计量比较(即,Pongoweidenreichi,PongoDevosi,Pongoduboisi,Pongopalaeosumatrensis,Pongojavensis,和Pongosp.)和现存的猩猩(即,Pongopygmaeus和Pongoabelii)。中山庞戈组合的年代采用U系列和耦合电子自旋共振/U系列方法。我们的结果合理地将中山庞戈组合限制在184±16ka,这与生物地层学证据是一致的。中山庞戈的牙齿平均仅比现有庞戈的牙齿大6.5%。中山牙齿总体上比中国南方所有其他洞穴遗址的Pongo牙齿小,它们目前代表了中国南方最小的猩猩化石。根据牙齿的大小,上下切牙上有发达的舌柱和舌带,上磨牙上舌扣带残留物的中频,上磨牙和下磨牙的中度至重度起皱频率更高,我们暂时将中山化石分配给P.devosi。我们的结果证实了先前的说法,即P.weidenreichi在中国南方被较小的物种取代,P.Devosi,到中更新世晚期。中山洞中紫花苜蓿的发生进一步扩展了其时空分布。中山的Pongo标本提供了重要的新证据,证明了中国南方Pongo的牙齿形态特征在中更新世晚期发生了实质性变化。
    The rarity of Pongo fossils with precise absolute dating from the Middle Pleistocene hampers our understanding of the taxonomy and spatiotemporal distribution of Quaternary orangutans in southern China. Here, we report a newly discovered sample of 113 isolated teeth of fossil Pongo from Zhongshan Cave in the Bubing Basin, Guangxi, southern China. We describe the Pongo specimens from Zhongshan Cave and compare them metrically to other samples of fossil Pongo species (i.e., Pongo weidenreichi, Pongo devosi, Pongo duboisi, Pongo palaeosumatrensis, Pongo javensis, and Pongo sp.) and to extant orangutans (i.e., Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii). The Zhongshan Pongo assemblage is dated using U-series and coupled electron spin resonance/U-series methods. Our results reasonably constrain the Zhongshan Pongo assemblage to 184 ± 16 ka, which is consistent with the biostratigraphic evidence. The Zhongshan Pongo teeth are only 6.5% larger on average than those of extant Pongo. The Zhongshan teeth are smaller overall than those of Pongo from all other cave sites in southern China, and they currently represent the smallest fossil orangutans in southern China. Based on their dental size, and the presence of a well-developed lingual pillar and lingual cingulum on the upper and lower incisors, an intermediate frequency of lingual cingulum remnants on the upper molars, and a higher frequency of moderate to heavy wrinkling on the upper and lower molars, we provisionally assign the Zhongshan fossils to P. devosi. Our results confirm earlier claims that P. weidenreichi is replaced by a smaller species in southern China, P. devosi, by the late Middle Pleistocene. The occurrence of P. devosi in Zhongshan Cave further extends its spatial and temporal distribution. The Pongo specimens from Zhongshan provide important new evidence to demonstrate that the dental morphological features of Pongo in southern China changed substantially during the late Middle Pleistocene.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:人类化石手法行为的重建通常依赖于现有人类手的比较分析,以了解手的使用与骨骼形态之间的关系。在这种情况下,中间指骨仍未得到充分研究。因此,在这里,我们研究了现有人类中间指骨的皮质骨形态,并将其与近端指骨的皮质结构进行比较,研究皮质骨结构与人工行为过程中推断的负荷之间的关系。
    方法:使用微CT数据,我们分析了Pongopygmaeus(n=6个人)的2-5指中间指骨的皮质骨结构,大猩猩(n=22),平底锅。(n=23),和智人(n=23)。R包形态图用于研究皮质骨分布,分类单元内和跨分类单元的皮质厚度和横截面特性。
    结果:非人类类人猿通常掌干上有厚的皮质骨,波戈只在屈鞘脊的峰上有厚的皮质,而非洲类人猿在整个屈鞘脊和滑车近端有厚的皮层。人类的独特之处在于背轴皮层较厚,并且在轴的掌侧区域具有较厚的皮层。
    结论:中间指骨的皮质骨分布和性质的变化与现存大猿的运动和操纵行为的差异一致。中间指骨和近端指骨之间的比较揭示了每个分类单元内皮质骨分布的相似模式,但近端指骨承受的负荷可能更大。甚至在指关节行走的非洲类人猿中。这项研究为化石人类和原始人类中习惯性手的重建提供了比较背景。
    Reconstruction of fossil hominin manual behaviors often relies on comparative analyses of extant hominid hands to understand the relationship between hand use and skeletal morphology. In this context, the intermediate phalanges remain understudied. Thus, here we investigate cortical bone morphology of the intermediate phalanges of extant hominids and compare it to the cortical structure of the proximal phalanges, to investigate the relationship between cortical bone structure and inferred loading during manual behaviors.
    Using micro-CT data, we analyze cortical bone structure of the intermediate phalangeal shaft of digits 2-5 in Pongo pygmaeus (n = 6 individuals), Gorilla gorilla (n = 22), Pan spp. (n = 23), and Homo sapiens (n = 23). The R package morphomap is used to study cortical bone distribution, cortical thickness and cross-sectional properties within and across taxa.
    Non-human great apes generally have thick cortical bone on the palmar shaft, with Pongo only having thick cortex on the peaks of the flexor sheath ridges, while African apes have thick cortex along the entire flexor sheath ridge and proximal to the trochlea. Humans are distinct in having thicker dorsal shaft cortex as well as thick cortex at the disto-palmar region of the shaft.
    Variation in cortical bone distribution and properties of the intermediate phalanges is consistent with differences in locomotor and manipulative behaviors in extant great apes. Comparisons between the intermediate and proximal phalanges reveals similar patterns of cortical bone distribution within each taxon but with potentially greater load experienced by the proximal phalanges, even in knuckle-walking African apes. This study provides a comparative context for the reconstruction of habitual hand use in fossil hominins and hominids.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号