Orientation, Spatial

方向,Spatial
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:单-或双侧前庭周围性损伤会导致客观的空间定向缺陷,可以使用笔和纸测试或感觉运动任务(导航或指向)进行测量。对于患者的主观定向能力,问卷是常用的(例如,圣巴巴拉方向感刻度[SBSODS])。然而,对空间技能的主观评估与客观前庭功能之间的关系几乎没有研究。
    方法:共177例患者(平均年龄57.86±17.53岁,90名女性)在我们的眩晕和平衡障碍三级中心就诊,接受了神经耳科检查,包括双热水热量,视频头脉冲测试(vHIT),和主观视觉垂直(SVV)的测试,并填写了SBSODS(德语版)。对前庭检测结果与自评评分进行相关性分析和线性多元回归模型分析。此外,分组前庭功能低的患者,平均,并分析了较高的自我报告分数。
    结果:42例患者符合双侧前庭病的诊断标准,93用于慢性单侧前庭病变(68例单侧热量功能减退和25例孤立的水平前庭眼反射缺陷),42例患者前庭检查结果正常。SBSODS评分显示出明显的性别差异,男性主观技能水平较高(平均得分为男性:4.94±0.99,女性4.40±0.94;学生t检验:t-3.78,p<.001***)。没有发现客观前庭功能与主观空间定向之间的稳定相关性。多元线性回归模型不能可靠地解释自我报告的方差。这三个患者组的低,平均,高的自我评估得分显示前庭功能没有显着差异。
    结论:自我报告的空间方向评估与客观的外周前庭功能并不密切相关。因此,在现实世界和虚拟环境中测量空间技能的其他方法需要揭示由于前庭功能减退导致的方位缺陷。
    BACKGROUND: Uni- or bilateral peripheralvestibular impairment causes objective spatial orientation deficits, which can be measured using pen-and-paper-tests or sensorimotor tasks (navigation or pointing). For patients\' subjective orientation abilities, questionnaires are commonly used (e.g., Santa Barbara sense of direction scale [SBSODS]). However, the relationship between subjective assessment of spatial skills and objective vestibular function has only been scarcely investigated.
    METHODS: A total of 177 patients (mean age 57.86 ± 17.53 years, 90 females) who presented in our tertiary Center for Vertigo and Balance Disorders underwent neuro-otological examinations, including bithermal water calorics, video head impulse test (vHIT), and testing of the subjective visual vertical (SVV), and filled out the SBSODS (German version). Correlation analyses and linear multiple regression model analyses were performed between vestibular test results and self-assessment scores. Additionally, groupwise vestibular function for patients with low, average, and high self-report scores was analyzed.
    RESULTS: Forty-two patients fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for bilateral vestibulopathy, 93 for chronic unilateral vestibulopathy (68 unilateral caloric hypofunction and 25 isolated horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex deficits), and 42 patients had normal vestibular test results. SBSODS scores showed clear sex differences with higher subjective skill levels in males (mean score males: 4.94 ± 0.99, females 4.40 ± 0.94; Student\'s t-test: t-3.78, p < .001***). No stable correlation between objective vestibular function and subjective sense of spatial orientation was found. A multiple linear regression model could not reliably explain the self-reported variance. The three patient groups with low, average, and high self-assessment-scores showed no significant differences of vestibular function.
    CONCLUSIONS: Self-reported assessment of spatial orientation does not robustly correlate with objective peripheral vestibular function. Therefore, other methods of measuring spatial skills in real-world and virtual environments are required to disclose orientation deficits due to vestibular hypofunction.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    昆虫的中心复合体包含细胞,组织成一个环吸引子,编码头部方向。环中活动的“凸起”可以通过白痴提示和外部感官信息来更新。这些细胞和环形神经元之间的突触可塑性,负责将感官信息带入中央建筑群,已被提议在视觉线索和航向估计之间形成映射,从而允许更准确地跟踪当前航向,而不是只使用白痴信息。在果蝇中,环状神经元具有良好表征的非线性感受野。在这项工作中,我们使用兴奋性输入和环形神经元之间的相互抑制相结合来生产这些视觉感受野的合成版本。基于最近发表的果蝇连接体,我们使用这些感受场将视觉信息带入昆虫中心复合体的尖峰神经网络模型。先前的建模工作集中在该电路如何使用实验上常用的相同类型的简单视觉线索作为环形吸引子。当我们最初在这些简单的刺激上测试模型时,然后,我们继续将模型应用于包含多个冲突线索的复杂自然场景。我们表明,环形神经元提供的这种简单的视觉滤波足以在航向和视觉特征之间形成映射,并在没有角速度输入的情况下保持航向估计。即使在呈现包含来自环境变化和相机平移的冲突信息的自然场景的视频时,网络也能成功地跟踪航向。
    The central complex of insects contains cells, organised as a ring attractor, that encode head direction. The \'bump\' of activity in the ring can be updated by idiothetic cues and external sensory information. Plasticity at the synapses between these cells and the ring neurons, that are responsible for bringing sensory information into the central complex, has been proposed to form a mapping between visual cues and the heading estimate which allows for more accurate tracking of the current heading, than if only idiothetic information were used. In Drosophila, ring neurons have well characterised non-linear receptive fields. In this work we produce synthetic versions of these visual receptive fields using a combination of excitatory inputs and mutual inhibition between ring neurons. We use these receptive fields to bring visual information into a spiking neural network model of the insect central complex based on the recently published Drosophila connectome. Previous modelling work has focused on how this circuit functions as a ring attractor using the same type of simple visual cues commonly used experimentally. While we initially test the model on these simple stimuli, we then go on to apply the model to complex natural scenes containing multiple conflicting cues. We show that this simple visual filtering provided by the ring neurons is sufficient to form a mapping between heading and visual features and maintain the heading estimate in the absence of angular velocity input. The network is successful at tracking heading even when presented with videos of natural scenes containing conflicting information from environmental changes and translation of the camera.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人类通过目标位置的视觉定位或通过注意力的秘密转移来定位他们的感知世界。定向为所选位置提供了首要性,并且在人类中提高了辨别的精度。隐蔽定向似乎与扫视眼球运动所涉及的机制分开产生。隐蔽定向可以用于优先处理目标,甚至增加其主观强度和敏锐度。然而,此网络似乎不涉及与绑定和分段相关的操作。细胞存在于早期视觉皮层中,它们被颜色和形态特征激活而无需注意,然而,即使指向需要报告的目标,颜色和形式也似乎保持独立。对将注意力网络连接到记忆网络的途径的理解可能使我们能够理解空间认知的更复杂方面,并增强定向,从而改善空间认知。
    Humans orient to their sensory world through foveation of target location or through covert shifts of attention. Orienting provides primacy to the selected location and in humans improves the precision of discrimination. Covert orienting appears to arise separately from the mechanisms involved in saccadic eye movements. Covert orienting can serve to prioritize processing the target even increasing its subjective intensity and its acuity. However, this network does not appear to be involved in the operations related to binding and segmentation. Cells exist in the early visual cortex that are activated by both color and form features without attention, however, color and form appear to remain independent even when oriented to the target that is required to be reported. An understanding of the pathways that connect attention networks to memory networks may allow us to understand more complex aspects of spatial cognition and enhance orienting and thus improve spatial cognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    依赖于极性的定向错觉构成了一类错觉,其中定向的印象不仅仅取决于其元素之间的几何关系,还有它们亮度之间的关系。本文介绍了这些数字的几个例子。Todorović(2021a)提出了这种现象的简单计算模型。该模型的模拟表明,这些错觉的神经基础的共同特征是存在某些称为“倾斜簇”的神经结构。该模型的局限性在于它使用了一组受限制的参数。在本文中,介绍了该模型的一般化,涉及接受域的类型,它们的方向灵敏度和它们的大小或空间频率调谐。新模型的模拟表明,在更广泛的条件下,反应模式中存在倾斜簇,虽然不是全部。证明了倾斜簇构成此类错觉中倾斜印象的神经基础的原始假设。
    Polarity-dependent orientation illusions constitute a class of illusions in which the impression of orientation does not depend only on geometrical relations between its elements, but also on the relations between their luminances. Several examples of such figures are presented in the paper. Todorović (2021a) presented a simple computational model of such phenomena. Simulations of the model indicated that a common feature of the neural basis of these illusions is the presence of certain neural structures called \'oblique clusters\'. A limitation of the model was that it used a restricted set of parameters. In this paper a generalization of the model is introduced involving types of receptive fields, their orientation sensitivity and their size or spatial frequency tuning. The simulations of the new model indicated that oblique clusters were present in the reaction patterns under a much wider set of conditions, though not all. The original hypothesis that oblique clusters constituted the neural foundations of impressions of tilt in this class of illusions was vindicated.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    感知垂直度对于准确的空间定位至关重要。先前的研究表明,倾斜的场景会使垂直度感知产生偏差。垂直感知偏差可以表示为在视觉取向感知中起作用的多个周期函数的总和,其中影响每个周期的具体因素仍然不确定。这项研究调查了室内场景的宽度和深度对偏差的每个周期性分量的影响。向参与者展示了一个室内场景,显示了一个矩形棋盘室(实验1),墙上的矩形孔(实验2),或一个矩形的点状房间(实验3),各种纵横比。刺激以顺时针90°至逆时针90°的滚动方向呈现。要求参与者报告他们的主观视觉垂直(SVV)感知。45°的贡献,90°,通过加权向量和模型评估SVV误差的180°周期性。在实验1中,SVV误差的周期性分量随纵横比的增加而增加。在实验2和3中,仅90°分量随纵横比增加。这些发现表明,延伸的横向表面可能会调节垂直感知的周期性成分。
    Perceiving verticality is crucial for accurate spatial orientation. Previous research has revealed that tilted scenes can bias verticality perception. Verticality perception bias can be represented as the sum of multiple periodic functions that play a role in the perception of visual orientation, where the specific factors affecting each periodicity remain uncertain. This study investigated the influence of the width and depth of an indoor scene on each periodic component of the bias. The participants were presented with an indoor scene showing a rectangular checkerboard room (Experiment 1), a rectangular aperture on the wall (Experiment 2), or a rectangular dotted room (Experiment 3), with various aspect ratios. The stimuli were presented with roll orientations ranging from 90° clockwise to 90° counterclockwise. The participants were asked to report their subjective visual vertical (SVV) perceptions. The contributions of 45°, 90°, and 180° periodicities to the SVV error were assessed by the weighted vector sum model. In Experiment 1, the periodic components of the SVV error increased with the aspect ratio. In Experiments 2 and 3, only the 90° component increased with the aspect ratio. These findings suggest that extended transverse surfaces may modulate the periodic components of verticality perception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    没有视觉参考,接触协调飞行的非飞行员低估了倾斜角,由于耳石的滚动角位移信息不一致,一致地发出垂直位置信号,相对于半规管,能够检测位移。飞行员还可以利用他们感知G负载的能力以及负载与角度之间关系的知识来评估倾斜角。我们的目的是研究是否可以通过离心机中的空间定向训练来改善倾斜角度的感知。16名飞行员/飞行员学生评估了他们的侧倾倾斜,在完全黑暗中,在真正的协调飞行转弯和吊舱离心期间,在30°和60°的辊倾斜。3周后重复实验,在此期间,8名受试者在离心机中进行了9次培训,包括滚动角与G负载的反馈,并指示所要求的角度。培训前,在飞机和离心机中分别感知到的受试者:37(17)°;在60°转弯中38(14)°,和19(12)°;20(10)°在30°转弯。在60°[至60(7)°;55(10)°;p≤0.04]期间,训练改善了对角度的感知,但不是30°转弯[21(10)°;15(9)°;p≥0.30];训练后两年内改善消失。未训练组的角度评估没有变化。结果表明,这是可能的,在离心机中,训练飞行员感知大的能力,但不是离散到中等的滚动角位移。瞬态训练效应可归因于感知G载荷并将其转化为滚动角的能力提高和/或对半圆形运河信号的依赖增加。
    Without visual references, nonpilots exposed to coordinated flight turns underestimate the bank angle, because of discordant information of the roll-angular displacement from the otoliths, consistently signaling vertical position, versus the semicircular canals, enabling detection of the displacement. Pilots may also use their ability to perceive the G load and knowledge of the relation between load and angle to assess the bank angle. Our aim was to investigate whether the perception of bank angle can be improved by spatial orientation training in a centrifuge. Sixteen pilots/pilot students assessed their roll tilt, in complete darkness, during both real coordinated flight turns and gondola centrifugation, at roll tilts of 30° and 60°. The experiments were repeated after a 3-wk period, during which eight of the subjects performed nine training sessions in the centrifuge, comprising feedback on roll angle vs. G load, and on indicating requested angles. Before training, the subjects perceived in the aircraft and centrifuge, respectively: 37 (17)°, 38 (14)° during 60° turns and 19 (12)°, 20 (10)° during 30° turns. Training improved the perception of angle during the 60° [to 60 (7)°, 55 (10)°; P ≤ 0.04] but not the 30° [21 (10)°, 15 (9)°; P ≥ 0.30] turns; the improvement disappeared within 2 yr after training. Angle assessments did not change in the untrained group. The results suggest that it is possible to, in a centrifuge, train a pilot\'s ability to perceive large but not discrete-to-moderate roll-angular displacements. The transient training effect is attributable to improved capacity to perceive and translate G load into roll angle and/or to increased reliance on semicircular canal signals.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Spatial disorientation is a major problem in aviation. When performing coordinated flight turns without external visual cues (e.g., flying in clouds or darkness), the pilot underestimates the aircraft bank angle because the vestibular system provides unreliable information of roll tilt. The present study demonstrates that it is possible to, in a long-arm centrifuge, train a pilot\'s ability to perceive large but not discrete-to-moderate roll-angular displacements.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    重新定向,失去后重新获得轴承的过程,需要识别空间上下文(上下文识别)并恢复该上下文中的面向方向(标题检索)。我们以前表明,这些过程依赖于使用特征和几何形状,分别。这里,我们研究了在长时间范围内产生上下文歧义的任务中的重新定向行为,以证明雄性小鼠学会结合特征和几何线索来恢复航向。在神经层面,大多数CA1神经元持续与几何体对齐,这种对齐方式可以预测航向行为。然而,一小部分细胞以上下文敏感的方式相干地重新映射,用来预测上下文。有效的标题检索和上下文识别与反映活动集合中特征和几何信息集成的速率变化相关。这些数据说明了如何在CA1中对上下文识别和标题检索进行编码,以及这些过程如何随经验而变化。
    Reorientation, the process of regaining one\'s bearings after becoming lost, requires identification of a spatial context (context recognition) and recovery of facing direction within that context (heading retrieval). We previously showed that these processes rely on the use of features and geometry, respectively. Here, we examine reorientation behavior in a task that creates contextual ambiguity over a long timescale to demonstrate that male mice learn to combine both featural and geometric cues to recover heading. At the neural level, most CA1 neurons persistently align to geometry, and this alignment predicts heading behavior. However, a small subset of cells remaps coherently in a context-sensitive manner, which serves to predict context. Efficient heading retrieval and context recognition correlate with rate changes reflecting integration of featural and geometric information in the active ensemble. These data illustrate how context recognition and heading retrieval are coded in CA1 and how these processes change with experience.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    区域V4是猕猴视觉皮层层次结构的中级区域,通过整合来自V1和V2等较低区域的多个功能隔室的前馈输入,并向下颞叶的许多区域提供前馈输入,在皮层视觉处理中发挥关键功能。顶叶,和前额叶皮层.虽然许多先前的V4成像研究已经分析了对颜色的差异响应,定位,视差,和运动刺激,许多细节的空间组织显著的色调和方向调整尚未得到充分描述。内在皮层单条件响应的支持向量机(SVM)解码用于生成V4中色调和方向调整的高分辨率图。与V1和V2一样,V4包含围绕风车中心组织的等向域图。V4包含由围绕风车中心的等色调域组成的色调图。与在先前的皮质区域中观察到的相比,这些风车的圆形组织更接近地代表了对色调的感知。色调显著调整的域占据取向域表面的大约四倍,很大程度上彼此隔离,重叠大约5%。色调和方向风车的空间组织及其域与V2的薄和条纹间隔室产生的大部分分离的输入基本一致。这种模块化的处理分离表明,颜色和形状的进一步整合必须发生在接受V4直接投射的下颞叶皮层区域。
    Area V4 is an intermediate-level area of the macaque visual cortical hierarchy that serves key functions in visual processing by integrating inputs from lower areas such as V1 and V2 and providing feedforward inputs to many higher cortical areas. Previous V4 imaging studies have focused on differential responses to color, orientation, disparity, and motion stimuli, but many details of the spatial organization of significant hue and orientation tuning have not been fully described. We used support vector machine (SVM) decoding of intrinsic cortical single-condition responses to generate high-resolution maps of hue and orientation tuning and to describe the organization of hue and orientation pinwheels in V4. Like V1 and V2, V4 contains maps of orientation that are organized as pinwheels. V4 also contains maps of hue that are organized as pinwheels, whose circular organization more closely represents the perception of hue than is observed in antecedent cortical areas. Unlike V1, where orientation is continuously mapped across the surface, V4 hue and orientation pinwheels are organized in limited numbers of pinwheel sequences. The organization of these sequences and the distance between pinwheels may provide insight into the functional organization of V4. Regions significantly tuned for hue occupy roughly four times that of the orientation, are largely separated from each other, and overlap by roughly 5%. This spatial organization is largely consistent with segregated inputs arising from V2 thin and interstripes. This modular organization of V4 suggests that further integration of color and shape might occur in higher areas in inferotemporal cortical.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The representation of hue and orientation in macaque monkey area V4 was determined by intrinsic cortical imaging of responses to isoluminant hues and achromatic grating stimuli. Vector summation of support vector machine (SVM) decoded single-condition responses was used to generate hue and orientation maps that, like V1 orientation maps, were both characterized by distinct pinwheel patterns. These data suggest that pinwheels are an important structure to represent different stimulus features across multiple visual cortical areas.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    鸟群的空中蜂拥,或杂音,在对行为研究和模拟提出许多挑战的同时,也使观察者着迷。我们研究了杂物的外围如何保持良好的边界和凝聚力。我们还研究了搅动波,当羊群受到干扰时,为它们如何自发出现开发一个合理的模型。为了理解这些行为,提出了一种新的基于定向的社会植绒模型。以前的方法通过考虑每只鸟周围的邻居来模拟鸟间的动态,引入回避力量,对齐,和内聚力作为改变加速度的三维矢量。我们的方法引入了基于定向的社会植绒,将邻居的社会影响更现实地视为转向的愿望,在空气动力学模型中间接控制航向。虽然我们的模型可以应用于任何成群的社交鸟类,但我们模拟了一群八哥,Sturusvulgaris,并证明了在没有捕食者的情况下发生定向波的可能性。我们的模型表现出与观察结果相匹配的球形和卵形群形状。我们的模型与雷诺在能耗和频率分析上的比较表明了更真实的运动,转弯时使用的能源明显减少,和出现定向波的合理机制。
    The aerial flocking of birds, or murmurations, has fascinated observers while presenting many challenges to behavioral study and simulation. We examine how the periphery of murmurations remain well bounded and cohesive. We also investigate agitation waves, which occur when a flock is disturbed, developing a plausible model for how they might emerge spontaneously. To understand these behaviors a new model is presented for orientation-based social flocking. Previous methods model inter-bird dynamics by considering the neighborhood around each bird, and introducing forces for avoidance, alignment, and cohesion as three dimensional vectors that alter acceleration. Our method introduces orientation-based social flocking that treats social influences from neighbors more realistically as a desire to turn, indirectly controlling the heading in an aerodynamic model. While our model can be applied to any flocking social bird we simulate flocks of starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, and demonstrate the possibility of orientation waves in the absence of predators. Our model exhibits spherical and ovoidal flock shapes matching observation. Comparisons of our model to Reynolds\' on energy consumption and frequency analysis demonstrates more realistic motions, significantly less energy use in turning, and a plausible mechanism for emergent orientation waves.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    大量文献研究了知觉学习的特殊性和迁移,暗示一幅复杂的画面。这里,我们区分“任务相关”功能中的变化之间的转移(例如,将学习的方向任务转移到不同的参考方向)并转移到“任务无关”特征(例如,将学习的方向任务转移到不同的视网膜位置或不同的空间频率),我们专注于后者的机制。实验上,我们评估是否使用不相关特征的一个值来学习对一个特征(如方向)的判断(例如,空间频率)转移到不相关特征的另一个值。实验1检查了具有一个或多个空间频率的八种替代方向识别中的学习是否转移到五个不同空间频率的刺激。实验2与实验1平行,研究了在一个或多个方向上进行八种替代空间频率识别的学习是否转移到具有五个不同方向的刺激。训练定向任务的单一空间频率广泛转移到所有其他空间频率,在以最高的空间频率训练时倾向于特异性。训练空间频率任务在所有方向上完全转移。计算上,我们扩展了识别综合重加权理论(I-IRT)来考虑传输数据(Dosher,Liu,&Lu,2023年;刘,Dosher,&Lu,2023年)。正如原始IRT中的位置不变表示解释视网膜位置的转移一样,结合特征不变表示有效地解释了观察到的转移。一起来看,我们建议特征不变表示可以解释“任务无关”特征的学习迁移。
    A large body of literature has examined specificity and transfer of perceptual learning, suggesting a complex picture. Here, we distinguish between transfer over variations in a \"task-relevant\" feature (e.g., transfer of a learned orientation task to a different reference orientation) and transfer over a \"task-irrelevant\" feature (e.g., transfer of a learned orientation task to a different retinal location or different spatial frequency), and we focus on the mechanism for the latter. Experimentally, we assessed whether learning a judgment of one feature (such as orientation) using one value of an irrelevant feature (e.g., spatial frequency) transfers to another value of the irrelevant feature. Experiment 1 examined whether learning in eight-alternative orientation identification with one or multiple spatial frequencies transfers to stimuli at five different spatial frequencies. Experiment 2 paralleled Experiment 1, examining whether learning in eight-alternative spatial-frequency identification at one or multiple orientations transfers to stimuli with five different orientations. Training the orientation task with a single spatial frequency transferred widely to all other spatial frequencies, with a tendency to specificity when training with the highest spatial frequency. Training the spatial frequency task fully transferred across all orientations. Computationally, we extended the identification integrated reweighting theory (I-IRT) to account for the transfer data (Dosher, Liu, & Lu, 2023; Liu, Dosher, & Lu, 2023). Just as location-invariant representations in the original IRT explain transfer over retinal locations, incorporating feature-invariant representations effectively accounted for the observed transfer. Taken together, we suggest that feature-invariant representations can account for transfer of learning over a \"task-irrelevant\" feature.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号