Numerical estimation

数值估计
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    解决数学问题时,当幼儿可以使用与心理表征相匹配的手势时,他们会表现得更好。然而,尽管它们在教育环境中的患病率越来越高,很少有研究在基于触摸屏的交互中探索这种效果。因此,我们调查了在进行连续数字行估计任务时,在触摸屏设备上拖动(执行与数字变化一致的连续手势)和敲击(涉及不一致的离散手势)对幼儿表现的影响.通过检查数字线估计的设置大小和位置的差异,我们还能够探索相同手势的优势效果的边界条件。我们使用了2(手势类型:拖动或点击)×2(设置大小:设置0-10或设置0-20)×2(位置:中点左侧或中点右侧)混合设计。总共招募了70名5岁和6岁的儿童(33名女孩),并随机分配到Drag或Tap组。我们发现,一致的手势(拖动)通常有助于改善触摸屏的性能,但具有边界条件。完成困难的估计时(大集合大小的右侧),Drag组更准确,对刺激的反应更快,和花费更多的时间操纵比点击组。这些发现表明,当儿童需要明确的脚手架时,一致的触摸屏手势有助于释放心理资源以进行战略调整,降低了数值估计的难度,并支持构建心理表征。
    When solving mathematical problems, young children will perform better when they can use gestures that match mental representations. However, despite their increasing prevalence in educational settings, few studies have explored this effect in touchscreen-based interactions. Thus, we investigated the impact on young children\'s performance of dragging (where a continuous gesture is performed that is congruent with the change in number) and tapping (involving a discrete gesture that is incongruent) on a touchscreen device when engaged in a continuous number line estimation task. By examining differences in the set size and position of the number line estimation, we were also able to explore the boundary conditions for the superiority effect of congruent gestures. We used a 2 (Gesture Type: drag or tap) × 2 (Set Size: Set 0-10 or Set 0-20) × 2 (Position: left of midpoint or right of midpoint) mixed design. A total of 70 children aged 5 and 6 years (33 girls) were recruited and randomly assigned to either the Drag or Tap group. We found that the congruent gesture (drag) generally facilitated better performance with the touchscreen but with boundary conditions. When completing difficult estimations (right side in the large set size), the Drag group was more accurate, responded to the stimulus faster, and spent more time manipulating than the Tap group. These findings suggest that when children require explicit scaffolding, congruent touchscreen gestures help to release mental resources for strategic adjustments, decrease the difficulty of numerical estimation, and support constructing mental representations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    对人类决策最突出的社会影响之一是整合,当感知信息含糊不清时,这一点更加突出。这个问题的贝叶斯最优解需要在从自我来源/内生和社会来源构建感知时,加权认知信息和感知信号的相对可靠性,分别。当前的研究调查了人类是否整合了统计数据(即,均值和方差)在估计数量时,以贝叶斯最优方式计算内生感知和社会信息。我们的结果表明,只有当群体估计更可靠(或“确定”)时,初始估计才会向群体均值调整。与参与者的内生指标不确定性相比。我们的结果支持Bayes最佳社会整合,同时也指向一种隐含形式的元认知。
    One of the most prominent social influences on human decision making is conformity, which is even more prominent when the perceptual information is ambiguous. The Bayes optimal solution to this problem entails weighting the relative reliability of cognitive information and perceptual signals in constructing the percept from self-sourced/endogenous and social sources, respectively. The current study investigated whether humans integrate the statistics (i.e., mean and variance) of endogenous perceptual and social information in a Bayes optimal way while estimating numerosities. Our results demonstrated adjustment of initial estimations toward group means only when group estimations were more reliable (or \"certain\"), compared to participants\' endogenous metric uncertainty. Our results support Bayes optimal social conformity while also pointing to an implicit form of metacognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    各种研究报告了音乐专业知识与增强的视觉空间和数学能力之间的关联。最近的一项工作测试了音乐家和非音乐家对纸牌数字错觉的敏感性,发现数字估计背后的感知偏差也受到长期音乐训练的影响。然而,音乐专业知识与定量估计的不同感知机制之间的潜在联系可能仅限于视觉模态或通用(即,模态独立)。我们通过开发纸牌错觉的声学版本来解决这个问题。专业音乐家和非音乐家收听钢琴和长号音符的音频文件录音,并被要求估计钢琴音符的数量。刺激被安排形成测试试验,钢琴和长号音符的排列方式形成纸牌图案,和对照试验,用随机定位的音符来评估它们在声学模态中的定量能力。在对照试验中,音乐家在数字估计上比非音乐家更准确。在虚幻模式的存在下,非音乐家与音乐家在尊重定期安排与随机排列的笔记。这表明,长期音乐训练与数值估计基础的不同感知机制之间的关联可能不仅限于视觉模态。然而,音乐家和非音乐家似乎都不容易受到原声版本的纸牌错觉的影响,这表明这种错觉的出现可能是刺激和任务依赖的。
    Various studies have reported an association between musical expertise and enhanced visuospatial and mathematical abilities. A recent work tested the susceptibility of musicians and nonmusicians to the Solitaire numerosity illusion finding that also perceptual biases underlying numerical estimation are influenced by long-term music training. However, the potential link between musical expertise and different perceptual mechanisms of quantitative estimation may be either limited to the visual modality or universal (i.e., modality independent). We addressed this question by developing an acoustic version of the Solitaire illusion. Professional musicians and nonmusicians listened to audio file recordings of piano and trombone notes and were required to estimate the number of piano notes. The stimuli were arranged to form test trials, with piano and trombone notes arranged in a way to form the Solitaire pattern, and control trials, with randomly located notes to assess their quantitative abilities in the acoustic modality. In the control trials, musicians were more accurate in numerical estimation than nonmusicians. In the presence of illusory patterns, nonmusicians differed from musicians in the esteem of regularly arranged vs. randomly arranged notes. This suggests that the association between long-term musical training and different perceptual mechanisms underlying numerical estimation may not be confined to the visual modality. However, neither musicians nor nonmusicians seemed to be susceptible to the acoustic version of the Solitaire illusion, suggesting that the emergence of this illusion may be stimulus and task-dependent.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们提出了一种通过利用锚定效应来实现更好的人群智慧的方法。在这种方法中,人们首先被要求做出比较判断,例如“一个月后,新的COVID-19感染的数量是大于还是小于10(或20万)?”在这个例子中,两个足够不同的锚(例如,\"10\"或\"200,000\")在比较判断中设置。经过这个比较判断,人们被要求做出自己的估计。然后汇总这些估计。我们假设使用这种方法的汇总估计将比没有锚表示的估计更准确。为了检验所提出方法的有效性,我们进行了三项研究:一项计算机模拟和两项行为实验(知觉刺激的数值估计和医生对新的COVID-19感染的估计).通过计算机模拟,我们可以确定所提出的方法是有效的情况。尽管所提出的方法并不总是有效的(例如,当一个小组可以做出相当准确的估计时),平均而言,所提出的方法更有可能实现更好的智慧人群。特别是,当一组无法做出准确的估计时(即,显示偏见,如高估或低估),所提出的方法可以更好地实现人群的智慧。行为实验的结果与计算机模拟结果一致。所提出的方法更好地实现了人群的智慧。我们讨论了锚定效应的新见解,以及从小组成员中引起不同意见的方法。
    We propose a method to achieve better wisdom of crowds by utilizing anchoring effects. In this method, people are first asked to make a comparative judgment such as \"Is the number of new COVID-19 infections one month later more or less than 10 (or 200,000)?\" As in this example, two sufficiently different anchors (e.g., \"10\" or \"200,000\") are set in the comparative judgment. After this comparative judgment, people are asked to make their own estimates. These estimates are then aggregated. We hypothesized that the aggregated estimates using this method would be more accurate than those without anchor presentation. To examine the effectiveness of the proposed method, we conducted three studies: a computer simulation and two behavioral experiments (numerical estimation of perceptual stimuli and estimation of new COVID-19 infections by physicians). Through computer simulations, we could identify situations in which the proposed method is effective. Although the proposed method is not always effective (e.g., when a group can make fairly accurate estimations), on average, the proposed method is more likely to achieve better wisdom of crowds. In particular, when a group cannot make accurate estimations (i.e., shows biases such as overestimation or underestimation), the proposed method can achieve better wisdom of crowds. The results of the behavioral experiments were consistent with the computer simulation findings. The proposed method achieved better wisdom of crowds. We discuss new insights into anchoring effects and methods for inducing diverse opinions from group members.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的研究表明,在面对冲突的大小维度时处理数字的能力是数字判断的一个关键方面,部分依赖于非数值维度的抑制。在这里我们报告,第一次,这些抑制性控制过程特定于相互冲突的大小维度。在82名成年人的小组中使用适应冲突适应范式的非符号数字比较任务,我们发现,只有在前面的不一致试验中冲突维度相同时,数值信息和非数值信息之间的一致效应才会降低.因此,对数字的关注涉及在特定信息水平上起作用的抑制性控制过程。这些结果有助于更好地表征数字认知中涉及的领域一般能力,并为数字感知和抑制控制之间的特定相互作用提供证据。
    Recent studies have shown that the ability to process number in the face of conflicting dimensions of magnitude is a crucial aspect of numerosity judgments, relying in part on the inhibition of the non-numerical dimensions. Here we report, for the first time, that these inhibitory control processes are specific to the conflicting dimension of magnitude. Using a non-symbolic numerical comparison task adapted to a conflict adaptation paradigm on a group of 82 adults, we show that congruency effects between numerical and non-numerical information were reduced only when the conflicting dimension was the same in the preceding incongruent trial. Attention to number thus involves inhibitory control processes acting at a specific level of information. These results contribute to better characterize the domain general abilities involved in numerical cognition, and provide evidence for a specific interaction between numerosity perception and inhibitory control.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    正如欧洲报告的结果表明,教育干预对于在早期发展数学能力并防止教育系统中广泛的数学学习失败是必要的。许多研究都认为,与数学相关的特定领域预测因子是符号和非符号的幅度比较,还有,数行估计。这项研究的目标是设计4个数字学习应用程序游戏,以训练数学学习的特定认知基础,以创建资源并促进这些技术在教育界的使用,并促进有效的科学转移并提高研究知名度。这项研究涉及193名年龄在57-79个月的学龄前儿童。在标准化数学能力评估测试中获得分数后,对3组进行了准实验设计,即,低绩效组(N=49),高性能组(N=21),对照组(N=123)。结果表明,以4个数字学习应用游戏为重点的训练,亚硝化,数字事实,估计任务提高了实验组的数值技能,与对照组相比。这项研究的意义是,一方面,为早期数学能力的教学提供了经过验证的技术工具。另一方面,这项研究支持其他关于认知前兆在数学表现中重要性的研究。
    Educational interventions are necessary to develop mathematical competence at early ages and prevent widespread mathematics learning failure in the education system as indicated by the results of European reports. Numerous studies agree that domain-specific predictors related to mathematics are symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude comparison, as well as, number line estimation. The goal of this study was to design 4 digital learning app games to train specific cognitive bases of mathematical learning in order to create resources and promote the use of these technologies in the educational community and to promote effective scientific transfer and increase the research visibility. This study involved 193 preschoolers aged 57-79 months. A quasi-experimental design was carried out with 3 groups created after scores were obtained in a standardised mathematical competence assessment test, i.e., low-performance group (N = 49), high-performance group (N = 21), and control group (N = 123). The results show that training with the 4 digital learning app games focusing on magnitude, subitizing, number facts, and estimation tasks improved the numerical skills of the experimental groups, compared to the control group. The implications of the study were, on the one hand, provided verified technological tools for teaching early mathematical competence. On the other hand, this study supports other studies on the importance of cognitive precursors in mathematics performance.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当前的研究检查了离散数值估计是否基于与体重和时间等连续幅度估计相同的认知过程。虽然数字量的口头估计具有或有度量单位(例如,一个cookie罐中可以容纳多少cookie?_X_cookie),时间和体重的估计不会(例如,洗澡水需要多长时间?_X_分钟/小时/秒)。因此,对后一类的估计还有另一个难度,需要广泛参与认知控制。在功能磁共振成像(fMRI)扫描期间,18名学生进行了三个估计类别的估计:数字,时间,和重量。估计引起了多个大脑区域的活动,主要:(1)视觉区域,包括双侧舌回),(2)顶叶区,包括左角回和右脑上回,和(3)额叶区域(扣带回和下额叶皮层)。连续的震级估计(主要是时间)产生的额叶活动与离散的数值估计不同,证明离散数值估计和连续幅度估计之间的大脑激活情况不同。右额中下回的活动水平与给予极端反应的倾向相关,表明右前额叶在估计中的重要性。
    The current study examined whether discrete numerical estimation is based on the same cognitive process as estimation of continuous magnitudes such as weight and time. While the verbal estimation of numerical quantities has a contingent unit of measurement (e.g., how many cookies fit in a cookie jar? _X_ cookies), estimation of time and weight does not (e.g., how much time does it take to fill a bath with water? _X_ minutes/hours/seconds). Therefore, estimation of the latter categories has another level of difficulty, requiring extensive involvement of cognitive control. During a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan, 18 students performed estimations with three estimation categories: number, time, and weight. Estimations elicited activity in multiple brain regions, mainly: (1) visual regions including bilateral lingual gyrus), (2) parietal regions including the left angular gyrus and right supramarginal gyrus, and (3) the frontal regions (cingulate gyrus and the inferior frontal cortex). Continuous magnitude estimations (mostly time) produced different frontal activity than discrete numerical estimations did, demonstrating different profiles of brain activations between discrete numerical estimations and estimations of continuous magnitudes. The activity level in the right middle and inferior frontal gyrus correlated with the tendency to give extreme responses, signifying the importance of the right prefrontal lobe in estimations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近在加密货币衍生品交易所开发的比特币期货和期权合约标志着比特币价格风险对冲新时代的开始。对这些工具的需求可以追溯到1987年的市场崩盘,当时投资者需要更好的方法通过期权保险来保护他们的投资组合。这些工具为交易提供了更大的灵活性,并有效地对冲了比特币价格的波动。违反常数波动率和Black-Scholes期权定价模型的对数正态假设导致了波动率微笑的发现,傻笑,或在期权市场中出现偏差。这些程式化的事实;也就是说,期权价格隐含的波动率微笑和隐含波动率,在几乎所有金融市场的期权文献中都有很好的记录。预计比特币期权也是如此。该研究的数据集基于在Deribit比特币期货和期权交易所交易的两个时间段的短期比特币期权(14天到期日),荷兰的加密货币衍生品交易所。将估计结果与基准Black-Scholes隐含波动率值进行比较,以进行准确性和效率分析。本研究有两个目的:(1)提供对比特币期权波动率微笑的洞察;(2)通过数值逼近技术估计比特币期权的隐含波动率,特别是牛顿拉夫森和二分法。实验结果表明,基于比特币期权数据存在波动率远期偏斜,比特币期权属于商品类资产。此外,NewtonRaphson和Bisection方法可以有效估计比特币期权的隐含波动率。然而,牛顿拉夫森预测技术的收敛速度比二分法快。
    The recently developed Bitcoin futures and options contracts in cryptocurrency derivatives exchanges mark the beginning of a new era in Bitcoin price risk hedging. The need for these tools dates back to the market crash of 1987, when investors needed better ways to protect their portfolios through option insurance. These tools provide greater flexibility to trade and hedge volatile swings in Bitcoin prices effectively. The violation of constant volatility and the log-normality assumption of the Black-Scholes option pricing model led to the discovery of the volatility smile, smirk, or skew in options markets. These stylized facts; that is, the volatility smile and implied volatilities implied by the option prices, are well documented in the option literature for almost all financial markets. These are expected to be true for Bitcoin options as well. The data sets for the study are based on short-dated Bitcoin options (14-day maturity) of two time periods traded on Deribit Bitcoin Futures and Options Exchange, a Netherlands-based cryptocurrency derivative exchange. The estimated results are compared with benchmark Black-Scholes implied volatility values for accuracy and efficiency analysis. This study has two aims: (1) to provide insights into the volatility smile in Bitcoin options and (2) to estimate the implied volatility of Bitcoin options through numerical approximation techniques, specifically the Newton Raphson and Bisection methods. The experimental results show that Bitcoin options belong to the commodity class of assets based on the presence of a volatility forward skew in Bitcoin option data. Moreover, the Newton Raphson and Bisection methods are effective in estimating the implied volatility of Bitcoin options. However, the Newton Raphson forecasting technique converges faster than does the Bisection method.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Operational momentum (OM) refers to the behavioral tendency to overestimate or underestimate the results of addition or subtraction, respectively. The cognitive mechanism of the OM effect and how it is related to the development of symbolic math abilities are not well understood. The current study examined whether individual differences in the OM effect are related to symbolic arithmetic abilities, number line estimation performance, and the space-magnitude association effect in young children. In this study, first-grade elementary school children manifested the OM effect during approximate addition and subtraction. Individual differences in the OM effect were not correlated with number line estimation error. Interestingly, children who showed a greater degree of the OM effect performed not worse, but better on the symbolic arithmetic task. In addition, the OM effect was correlated with the space-magnitude association (size congruity) effect measured with the Numerical Stroop task. More specifically, the OM bias was correlated with the ability to inhibit interference from competing information on the incongruent trials of the Numerical Stroop task. Our results suggest that the inaccuracy of numerical magnitude representations is not the source of the OM effect. Given that children with better math ability showed a greater OM bias, a stronger OM effect may reflect better intuition in arithmetic operations. Altogether, we carefully interpret these findings as suggesting that a greater OM effect reflects superior intuition or fundamental knowledge of arithmetic operations and a more adult-like maturation of the reorienting component of the attentional system.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    subsizing是指人们能够准确,毫不费力地列举少量项目,大约有四个元素的容量。先前的研究表明,“规范”组织,比如骰子上熟悉的布局,可以很容易地提高人们的亚硝化表现。然而,世界上几乎所有的规范形状也是高度对称的;因此,目前还不清楚以前报道的规范组织的促进作用是否真的是由于规范,或者简单地由空间对称性驱动。这里,我们通过使用对称,然而非规范,形状结构。将这些对称布局与高度受控的随机图案进行比较(实验1),以及完全随机和规范模式(实验2)。我们的结果表明,对称性有助于降低性能,但仅在设定大小为6时,表明效果不足以改善较低或较高范围的人的表现。这也是真的,虽然较弱,在反应时间(RT),误差距离度量,韦伯分数。另一方面,规范布局在多个集合大小上产生了更快,更准确的subitizing性能。我们的结论是,尽管先前的发现在其规范形状中混合了对称性,他们关于形状规范的发现不能仅仅用对称性来解释。我们还建议我们的对称和规范的结果最好用“成群”和模式识别帐户来解释,分别。
    Subitizing refers to ability of people to accurately and effortlessly enumerate a small number of items, with a capacity around four elements. Previous research showed that \"canonical\" organizations, such as familiar layouts on a dice, can readily improve subitizing performance of people. However, almost all canonical shapes found in the world are also highly symmetrical; therefore, it is unclear whether previously reported facilitative effect of canonical organization is really due to canonicality, or simply driven by spatial symmetry. Here, we investigated the possible effect of symmetry on subitizing by using symmetrical, yet non-canonical, shape structures. These symmetrical layouts were compared with highly controlled random patterns (Experiment 1), as well as fully random and canonical patterns (Experiment 2). Our results showed that symmetry facilitates subitizing performance, but only at set size of 6, suggesting that the effect is insufficient to improve performance of people in the lower or upper range. This was also true, although weaker, in reaction time (RT), error distance measures, and Weber Fractions. On the other hand, canonical layouts produced faster and more accurate subitizing performances across multiple set sizes. We conclude that, although previous findings mixed symmetry in their canonical shapes, their findings on shape canonicality cannot be explained by symmetry alone. We also propose that our symmetrical and canonical results are best explained by the \"groupitizing\" and pattern recognition accounts, respectively.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号