Numerical development

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的证据表明,使用基于手指的策略有利于获得基本的数字技能。基本上有两种基于手指的策略需要区分:(a)手指计数(即,连续延伸单指)和(b)手指数字手势(即,同时延伸手指以表示量值)。在这项研究中,我们调查了3至5岁(N=156)儿童的自发和提示手指计数和手指数字手势以及它们对基本数字技能的贡献。结果显示,当被要求命名特定数量的动物时,只有6%的儿童自发地用手指进行计数,而59%的人使用手指数字手势来显示他们的年龄。这表明基于手指的策略的自发使用在很大程度上取决于特定的上下文。此外,儿童在提示手指计数方面的表现明显优于手指数字手势,这表明两种策略都是建立在一起的。最后,提示手指计数和手指数字手势明显和单独预测计数,基数知识,和基本算术。这些结果表明,手指计数和手指数字手势跟随并与数字发展呈正相关。
    Recent evidence suggests that using finger-based strategies is beneficial for the acquisition of basic numerical skills. There are basically two finger-based strategies to be distinguished: (a) finger counting (i.e., extending single fingers successively) and (b) finger number gesturing (i.e., extending fingers simultaneously to represent magnitudes). In this study, we investigated both spontaneous and prompted finger counting and finger number gesturing as well as their contribution to basic numerical skills in 3- to 5-year-olds (N = 156). Results revealed that only 6% of children spontaneously used their fingers for counting when asked to name a specific number of animals, whereas 59% applied finger number gesturing to show their age. This indicates that the spontaneous use of finger-based strategies depends heavily on the specific context. Moreover, children performed significantly better in prompted finger counting than in finger number gesturing, suggesting that both strategies build on each other. Finally, both prompted finger counting and finger number gesturing significantly and individually predicted counting, cardinal number knowledge, and basic arithmetic. These results indicate that finger counting and finger number gesturing follow and positively relate to numerical development.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    1974年罗杰·斯佩里,根据他对裂脑状况的开创性研究,得出结论,数学几乎完全由语言主导的左半球维持。右半球可以进行总计小于20的加法,这是完全左半球优势的唯一例外。对局部病变的研究得出了类似的结论,除了书面的复杂计算,根据计算程序的具体要求,需要空间能力在正确的位置显示数字。五十年后,新的理论和工具工具的贡献导致了一幅更加复杂的图景,据此,虽然大多数功能都证实了左半球在数学中的优势,几个数学相关的任务似乎是在右半球进行。数学函数偏侧化的发展轨迹也得到了阐明。这里回顾了这个知识语料库。当计算需要通用空间处理时,右半球并不简单地提供其支持,但它的作用可能非常具体。例如,右顶叶似乎存储了复杂的算术程序所需的特定于操作的空间布局,并且在解析包含零的复数时需要像右脑岛这样的区域。即使是简单的任务,也有证据表明两个半球之间存在复杂的编排:每个半球都有其特定的作用,同意正确的结果。至于发展,数据指向基本数值过程的正确优势。在学龄期出现的图片是一种双边模式,右半球的参与程度明显更高,特别是在非象征性任务中。顶内沟在这个年龄段对象征性刺激的反应中表现出左半球优势。
    In 1974, Roger Sperry, based on his seminal studies on the split-brain condition, concluded that math was almost exclusively sustained by the language dominant left hemisphere. The right hemisphere could perform additions up to sums less than 20, the only exception to a complete left hemisphere dominance. Studies on lateralized focal lesions came to a similar conclusion, except for written complex calculation, where spatial abilities are needed to display digits in the right location according to the specific requirements of calculation procedures. Fifty years later, the contribution of new theoretical and instrumental tools lead to a much more complex picture, whereby, while left hemisphere dominance for math in the right-handed is confirmed for most functions, several math related tasks seem to be carried out in the right hemisphere. The developmental trajectory in the lateralization of math functions has also been clarified. This corpus of knowledge is reviewed here. The right hemisphere does not simply offer its support when calculation requires generic space processing, but its role can be very specific. For example, the right parietal lobe seems to store the operation-specific spatial layout required for complex arithmetical procedures and areas like the right insula are necessary in parsing complex numbers containing zero. Evidence is found for a complex orchestration between the two hemispheres even for simple tasks: each hemisphere has its specific role, concurring to the correct result. As for development, data point to right dominance for basic numerical processes. The picture that emerges at school age is a bilateral pattern with a significantly greater involvement of the right-hemisphere, particularly in non-symbolic tasks. The intraparietal sulcus shows a left hemisphere preponderance in response to symbolic stimuli at this age.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在学校正式教学之前,孩子们似乎有一些算术能力,但是人们对这些能力的程度以及它们背后的机制知之甚少。在两项研究中,美国学龄前儿童的初步探索性研究(N=207;年龄=2.89-4.30岁)和意大利学龄前儿童的预注册复制(N=130;年龄=3-6.33岁),我们使用非符号减法任务记录了精确算术的一些基本行为签名。此外,我们通过分析精确减法中的个体差异与其他数值和非数值能力评估之间的关系,研究了潜在的机制。在这两项研究中,孩子们在确切的非符号算术任务上表现得高于偶然性,与涉及较大数量的问题相比,通常在涉及较小数量的问题上表现更好。此外,非语言近似数值能力和精确基数知识的个体差异与减法表现的不同方面有关。具体来说,非语言近似数值能力与年龄较大但年龄较小的儿童的减法表现相关。在这两项研究中,我们发现了基数知识与答案为零的减法问题的性能相关的证据(即,减法否定问题)。此外,只有对基数有基本了解的儿童才能解决减法否定问题。这些发现共同表明,核心的非语言数字能力,以及符号数字的新兴知识为一些人提供了基础,虽然有限,正规教育前的精确算术能力。
    Children appear to have some arithmetic abilities before formal instruction in school, but the extent of these abilities as well as the mechanisms underlying them are poorly understood. Over two studies, an initial exploratory study of preschool children in the U.S. (N = 207; Age = 2.89-4.30 years) and a pre-registered replication of preschool children in Italy (N = 130; Age = 3-6.33 years), we documented some basic behavioral signatures of exact arithmetic using a non-symbolic subtraction task. Furthermore, we investigated the underlying mechanisms by analyzing the relationship between individual differences in exact subtraction and assessments of other numerical and non-numerical abilities. Across both studies, children performed above chance on the exact non-symbolic arithmetic task, generally showing better performance on problems involving smaller quantities compared to those involving larger quantities. Furthermore, individual differences in non-verbal approximate numerical abilities and exact cardinal number knowledge were related to different aspects of subtraction performance. Specifically, non-verbal approximate numerical abilities were related to subtraction performance in older but not younger children. Across both studies we found evidence that cardinal number knowledge was related to performance on subtraction problems where the answer was zero (i.e., subtractive negation problems). Moreover, subtractive negation problems were only solved above chance by children who had a basic understanding of cardinality. Together these finding suggest that core non-verbal numerical abilities, as well as emerging knowledge of symbolic numbers provide a basis for some, albeit limited, exact arithmetic abilities before formal schooling.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    语法在数值发展中的作用,尤其是语法数字变形的作用,在幼儿时期已经有据可查了。不清楚,然而,语法语言结构的影响是否进一步扩展到更复杂的数字发展的后期阶段。这里,我们通过利用波兰人之间的差异来解决这个问题,具有复杂的语法数字范式,导致数字量和语法数之间的部分不一致的映射,德语,它具有相对简单的语言范式:使用象征性的数字比较任务对151名讲波兰语的幼儿园儿童和123名讲德语的幼儿园儿童进行了测试。此外,计数技能(给出数字和计数列表),以及数字的非符号(点集)和符号表示之间的映射,以及工作记忆(Corsi块和数字跨度)进行了评估。基于“赋予数字”和“映射”任务,孩子们被分为子集知识者,CP-knowers-non-mappers,和CP-knowers-mappers。语言背景在几个方面与表现有关:讲波兰语的儿童比德国儿童更晚地发展到CP-knowers阶段,尽管具有可比的非数值能力,即使在达到这个阶段之后,他们在数字比较任务中表现更差。波兰和德国群体之间的空间数字映射也存在有意义的差异。我们的发现符合语法数字范式影响的理论。数字表示的发展和处理,不仅在获取第一个数字词的含义的阶段,而且在稍后的阶段,在处理符号数字时。
    The role of grammar in numerical development, and particularly the role of grammatical number inflection, has already been well-documented in toddlerhood. It is unclear, however, whether the influence of grammatical language structure further extends to more complex later stages of numerical development. Here, we addressed this question by exploiting differences between Polish, which has a complex grammatical number paradigm, leading to a partially inconsistent mapping between numerical quantities and grammatical number, and German, which has a comparatively easy verbal paradigm: 151 Polish-speaking and 123 German-speaking kindergarten children were tested using a symbolic numerical comparison task. Additionally, counting skills (Give-a-Number and count-list), and mapping between non-symbolic (dot sets) and symbolic representations of numbers, as well as working memory (Corsi blocks and Digit span) were assessed. Based on the Give-a-Number and mapping tasks, the children were divided into subset-knowers, CP-knowers-non-mappers, and CP-knowers-mappers. Linguistic background was related to performance in several ways: Polish-speaking children expectedly progressed to the CP-knowers stage later than German children, despite comparable non-numerical capabilities, and even after this stage was achieved, they fared worse in the numerical comparison task. There were also meaningful differences in spatial-numerical mapping between the Polish and German groups. Our findings are in line with the theory that grammatical number paradigms influence. the development of representations and processing of numbers, not only at the stage of acquiring the meaning of the first number-words but at later stages as well, when dealing with symbolic numbers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    数字线估计任务已成为数字认知研究中最重要的方法之一。最初用作空间数表示的直接度量,它还提供了有关数字处理和相关策略的其他各个方面的信息。然而,这项工作和相关结论的大部分涉及以符号格式处理数字,由学童和年龄较大的科目。符号数字系统在学校正式教学和培训,及其基本数学属性(例如,等距,序)可以轻松地转换为定向数字线的空间格式。在孩子们完全熟悉符号数字系统之前,这就引发了关于数字线估计的基本特征的问题,即,当他们主要依靠非符号量的近似系统时。在我们的三项研究中,因此,我们研究学龄前儿童(3-5岁)如何估计非符号量在一条线上的位置,以及这种估计与发展中的符号数知识和文化(从左到右)方向性有关。孩子们接受了给数字任务的测试,然后他们执行了一个计算机化的数字线任务。在实验1中,使用了由1和20个元素组成的集合从左到右或从右到左的线。即使在数字能力最差的群体中,线性模型比对数或循环功率模型更好地拟合估计值。线方向无关紧要。在实验2中,使用1-9个从左到右取向的线。在群体水平上发现了线性模型的优势,估计的方差与测试的数量相关。在实验3中,位置到数字的过程再次揭示了线性模型的优势,尽管选择更接近行尾的选项的策略很普遍。在所有三个实验中,估计的精度随着对计数原理的掌握而增加。这些结果与基本数值表示的发展中对数到线性移位的假设相矛盾,而是支持具有标量方差的线性模型。然而,重要的问题仍然是数字线任务是否捕获了基本数字表示的性质,或者更确切地说,将该表示映射到外部空间的策略。
    The number-line estimation task has become one of the most important methods in numerical cognition research. Originally applied as a direct measure of spatial number representation, it became also informative regarding various other aspects of number processing and associated strategies. However, most of this work and associated conclusions concerns processing numbers in a symbolic format, by school children and older subjects. Symbolic number system is formally taught and trained at school, and its basic mathematical properties (e.g., equidistance, ordinality) can easily be transferred into a spatial format of an oriented number line. This triggers the question on basic characteristics of number line estimation before children get fully familiar with the symbolic number system, i.e., when they mostly rely on approximate system for non-symbolic quantities. In our three studies, we examine therefore how preschool children (3-5-years old) estimate position of non-symbolic quantities on a line, and how this estimation is related to the developing symbolic number knowledge and cultural (left-to-right) directionality. The children were tested with the Give-a-number task, then they performed a computerized number-line task. In Experiment 1, lines bounded with sets of 1 and 20 elements going left-to-right or right-to-left were used. Even in the least numerically competent group, the linear model better fit the estimates than the logarithmic or cyclic power models. The line direction was irrelevant. In Experiment 2, a 1-9 left-to-right oriented line was used. Advantage of linear model was found at group level, and variance of estimates correlated with tested numerosities. In Experiment 3, a position-to-number procedure again revealed the advantage of the linear model, although the strategy of selecting an option more similar to the closer end of the line was prevalent. The precision of estimation increased with the mastery of counting principles in all three experiments. These results contradict the hypothesis of the log-to-linear shift in development of basic numerical representation, rather supporting the linear model with scalar variance. However, the important question remains whether the number-line task captures the nature of the basic numerical representation, or rather the strategies of mapping that representation to an external space.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在数字认知研究中,操作动量(OM)现象(倾向于高估加法和/或绑定加法的结果到右侧,而低估减法和/或将其绑定到左侧)可以帮助阐明心算的最基本表示和过程及其发展。这项研究是首次在学龄前儿童中演示OM的符号算术。它以Haman和Lipowska(2021)的非符号算术任务为模型,使用阿拉伯数字而不是视觉集。了解阿拉伯数字和计数原理(CP)的77名儿童(4-7岁),但是没有事先的学校数学教育,解决了以视频形式呈现的加法和减法问题,其中一个作为第二个操作数。原则上,当涉及非符号近似数处理系统时,这些问题可能会很困难,而在符号格式中,它们可以仅基于后继/前继函数和数字顺序的知识来求解,不参考数值大小的表示。然而,参与者犯了系统性错误,特别是,高估加法的结果与典型的OM趋势一致。此外,减法和加法在向左和向右运动时引起更长的响应时间,分别,对应于OM的反向空间形式。这些结果在很大程度上复制了非符号任务的结果,并表明掌握符号算术的早期阶段的孩子可能依赖于数值幅度处理和空间数字关联,而不是新掌握的CP和精确数字的概念。
    In numerical cognition research, the operational momentum (OM) phenomenon (tendency to overestimate the results of addition and/or binding addition to the right side and underestimating subtraction and/or binding it to the left side) can help illuminate the most basic representations and processes of mental arithmetic and their development. This study is the first to demonstrate OM in symbolic arithmetic in preschoolers. It was modeled on Haman and Lipowska\'s (2021) non-symbolic arithmetic task, using Arabic numerals instead of visual sets. Seventy-seven children (4-7 years old) who know Arabic numerals and counting principles (CP), but without prior school math education, solved addition and subtraction problems presented as videos with one as the second operand. In principle, such problems may be difficult when involving a non-symbolic approximate number processing system, whereas in symbolic format they can be solved based solely on the successor/predecessor functions and knowledge of numerical orders, without reference to representation of numerical magnitudes. Nevertheless, participants made systematic errors, in particular, overestimating results of addition in line with the typical OM tendency. Moreover, subtraction and addition induced longer response times when primed with left- and right-directed movement, respectively, which corresponds to the reversed spatial form of OM. These results largely replicate those of non-symbolic task and show that children at early stages of mastering symbolic arithmetic may rely on numerical magnitude processing and spatial-numerical associations rather than newly-mastered CP and the concept of an exact number.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    假设数值大小信息在空间上以相对于以身体为中心定义的心理数字线的形式表示,自我中心的参照系。在这种情况下,空间语言技能,如掌握空间位置的口头描述(例如,在前面,后面,向右/向左)被认为与掌握心理数线上的数值之间的空间关系有关。我们检查了4至5岁儿童在允许以自我为中心和以分配为中心的参考框架中做出反应的任务中的空间语言技能,以及他们对数值大小的相对理解(通过数字单词比较任务评估)。此外,我们评估了儿童对数字大小的绝对理解的影响,这些理解是通过他们的数字单词理解(用手指计算不同的数字)和他们对数字序列的知识(确定前任和后继者以及识别序列的缺失骰子模式)来评估的。结果表明,当考虑与自我中心观点相对应的回应时,儿童的空间语言与他们的相对数值理解显著相关,即使在控制了协变量之后,如儿童SES,心理旋转技能,以及对数字序列的绝对幅度理解或知识。这表明,在空间语言中使用以自我为中心的参考框架可能有助于沿心理数字线进行数字的空间表示,因此对于学龄前儿童对数值大小的相对理解似乎很重要。
    Numerical magnitude information is assumed to be spatially represented in the form of a mental number line defined with respect to a body-centred, egocentric frame of reference. In this context, spatial language skills such as mastery of verbal descriptions of spatial position (e.g., in front of, behind, to the right/left) have been proposed to be relevant for grasping spatial relations between numerical magnitudes on the mental number line. We examined 4- to 5-year-old\'s spatial language skills in tasks that allow responses in egocentric and allocentric frames of reference, as well as their relative understanding of numerical magnitude (assessed by a number word comparison task). In addition, we evaluated influences of children\'s absolute understanding of numerical magnitude assessed by their number word comprehension (montring different numbers using their fingers) and of their knowledge on numerical sequences (determining predecessors and successors as well as identifying missing dice patterns of a series). Results indicated that when considering responses that corresponded to the egocentric perspective, children\'s spatial language was associated significantly with their relative numerical magnitude understanding, even after controlling for covariates, such as children\'s SES, mental rotation skills, and also absolute magnitude understanding or knowledge on numerical sequences. This suggests that the use of egocentric reference frames in spatial language may facilitate spatial representation of numbers along a mental number line and thus seem important for preschoolers\' relative understanding of numerical magnitude.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    数值发展的综合理论为理解数值发展提供了统一的方法,包括获取关于整数的知识,分数,小数,百分比,底片,以及所有这些类型的数字之间的关系(Siegler,汤普森,&Schneider,2011).虽然,在这种整合的许多方面都取得了相当大的进展(Siegler,我,席勒,田,&Braithwaite,2020),与其他类型的数字相比,百分比的作用受到的关注要少得多。本章旨在通过报告有关百分比及其与其他类型数字的关系的理解数据来纠正这种不平衡。我们首先描述了集成理论;然后总结了关于整数理解发展的已知知识,分数,和小数;然后描述在理解百分比作用方面的最新进展;最后考虑理论和研究的教学意义。
    The integrated theory of numerical development provides a unified approach to understanding numerical development, including acquisition of knowledge about whole numbers, fractions, decimals, percentages, negatives, and relations among all of these types of numbers (Siegler, Thompson, & Schneider, 2011). Although, considerable progress has been made toward many aspects of this integration (Siegler, Im, Schiller, Tian, & Braithwaite, 2020), the role of percentages has received much less attention than that of the other types of numbers. This chapter is an effort to redress this imbalance by reporting data on understanding of percentages and their relations to other types of numbers. We first describe the integrated theory; then summarize what is known about development of understanding of whole numbers, fractions, and decimals; then describe recent progress in understanding the role of percentages; and finally consider instructional implications of the theory and research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    这项研究调查了受教育程度和阿拉伯语数字的句法表示对从听写中转码两位数字的任务的影响。参与者是主要的,初中,高中生和高等教育学生。所有人都以阿拉伯语为母语。他们执行了一个转码任务,即从听写中写出两位数的数字。单元优先\\几十年第一写作模式是根据所指示的两位数数字的差异句法结构收集的(几十年第一:整十;单元第一:青少年数字;相同的单位和几十年,剩余的两位数数字)。调查结果显示,总的来说,讲阿拉伯语的人对两位数的数字采用了数十年的写作模式,特别是当它与两位数的句法结构一致时,就像整个几十个数字一样。这种十年第一的写作模式在初中更加明显和一致,高中,由于数学技能和第二和第三语言的提高,教育程度高于小学。然而,这种模式是由单位-十年结构的句法复杂性调制的。这种复杂性在处理更依赖于数字语法的两位数数字中更为明显。因此,整数,青少年数字,和相同的十年单位数字比其余的两位数字复杂。
    This study investigated the effect of educational level and of the syntactic representation of numbers in Arabic on the task of transcoding two-digit numbers from dictation. The participants were primary, junior-high, and high school pupils and higher education students. All spoke Arabic as a mother tongue. They performed a transcoding task, namely writing two-digit numbers from dictation. Units first\\decades first writing patterns were collected depending on the differential syntactic structures of the two-digit number dictated (decades first: whole tens; units first: teen numbers; identical units and decades, remaining two-digit numbers). The findings reveal that in general, Arabic speakers adopt a decades-first writing pattern for two-digit numbers, especially when it is consistent with the syntactic structure of two-digit numbers, as in whole-tens numbers. This decade-first writing pattern is more evident and consistent in junior-high school, high school, and higher education than in primary school due to the improvement in mathematical skills and second and third languages. However, this pattern is modulated by the syntactic complexity of the unit-decade structure. This complexity is more pronounced in two-digit numbers whose processing is more dependent on numerical syntax. Thus, whole-tens numbers, teen numbers, and identical-decade-unit numbers are less complex than the remaining two-digit numbers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Understanding fractions and decimals requires not only understanding each notation separately, or within-notation knowledge, but also understanding relations between notations, or cross-notation knowledge. Multiple notations pose a challenge for learners but could also present an opportunity, in that cross-notation knowledge could help learners to achieve a better understanding of rational numbers than could easily be achieved from within-notation knowledge alone. This hypothesis was tested by reanalyzing three published datasets involving fourth- to eighth-grade children from the United States and Finland. All datasets included measures of rational number arithmetic, within-notation magnitude knowledge (e.g., accuracy in comparing fractions vs. fractions and decimals vs. decimals), and cross-notation magnitude knowledge (e.g., accuracy in comparing fractions vs. decimals). Consistent with the hypothesis, cross-notation magnitude knowledge predicted fraction and decimal arithmetic when controlling for within-notation magnitude knowledge. Furthermore, relations between within-notation magnitude knowledge and arithmetic were not notation specific; fraction magnitude knowledge did not predict fraction arithmetic more than decimal arithmetic, and decimal magnitude knowledge did not predict decimal arithmetic more than fraction arithmetic. Implications of the findings for assessing rational number knowledge and learning and teaching about rational numbers are discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

公众号