Negative affect

负面影响
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    人们越来越有兴趣理解与焦虑有关的积极情感功能障碍,包括担心。这两个研究检查了情感表达抑制(研究1中的一般情感表达,研究2中的积极情感表达)和担忧之间的关联。对体验积极影响的倾向的调节作用特别感兴趣。受试者是居住在美国的成年人(研究1N=502,研究2N=250),他们通过众包网站招募并完成了研究变量的自我报告测量。适度回归用于检查研究预测,当加上积极影响减少时,影响表达将与担忧负相关。在多变量分析中,将对负面影响的倾向作为协变量。研究2(积极影响表达)支持影响表达和积极影响之间的相互作用效应,但研究1(一般影响表达)与担忧相关。表达积极影响的趋势减弱与更大的担忧相关,而积极影响较小。根据与担忧有关的积极情感表达的潜在情绪调节功能,讨论了研究结果和未来方向。
    There is emerging interest in understanding positive affect dysfunction in relation to anxiety, including worry. This set of two studies examined the association between the inhibition of affect expression (general affect expressivity in Study 1, positive affect expressivity in Study 2) and worry, with a particular interest in the moderating role of proneness to experience positive affect. Subjects were US-residing adults (Study 1 N = 502, Study 2 N = 250) who were recruited through a crowdsourcing website and completed self-report measures of study variables. Moderated regression was used to examine study predictions that affect expression would negatively correlate with worry when coupled with diminished positive affect. Proneness toward negative affect was included as a covariate within multivariate analyses. An interactive effect between affect expressivity and positive affect was supported in Study 2 (positive affect expressivity) but not Study 1 (general affect expressivity) in relation to worry. A diminished tendency to express positive affect was associated with greater worry when coupled with less positive affect. Study results and future directions are discussed in terms of the potential emotion-regulatory functions of positive affect expressivity as it relates to worry.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    动态情感过程的改变与情绪失调和抑郁有关。尽管抑郁症通常与惯性增强有关(即,更大的时刻到时刻的相关性)和可变性(即,与典型水平的较大偏离)成人的影响,对于有抑郁症风险的年轻人中是否存在改变的情感动力学,人们知之甚少。这项研究调查了处于不同抑郁风险的青年样本中临床抑郁和抑郁风险与积极和消极影响的惯性和变异性的关系。我们的样本包括147名14至17岁的青少年,分为三组:从未抑郁的低风险,从未抑郁的高风险(基于母亲抑郁史),和目前抑郁的青少年。青少年一周内每天最多完成7次正面和负面影响的生态瞬时评估。使用多水平模型和ANOVA来检查情感惯性和变异性与基于母亲史的青少年抑郁和风险的关联。控制平均影响。抑郁的青少年表现出更多的惰性和减少的积极影响,与风险较低和较高的青年相比,负面影响更加多变和升高,尽管在控制平均影响后关联减弱。从未抑郁的高风险和低风险青年之间没有差异。需要进行其他纵向研究,以评估日常生活中改变的影响动态是否先于抑郁症发作,以了解其在制定预防性干预措施中的作用。
    Alterations in dynamic affective processes are associated with dysregulated affect and depression. Although depression is often associated with heightened inertia (i.e., greater moment-to-moment correlation) and variability (i.e., larger departures from typical levels) of affect in adults, less is known about whether altered affect dynamics are present in youth at risk for depression. This study investigated the association of clinical depression and depression risk with the inertia and variability of positive and negative affect in a sample of youth at varying risk for depression. Our sample included 147 adolescents aged 14 to 17, categorized into three groups: never-depressed lower-risk, never-depressed higher-risk (based on maternal history of depression), and currently depressed adolescents. Adolescents completed ecological momentary assessments of positive and negative affect up to seven times per day for a week. Multilevel models and ANOVAs were used to examine associations of affective inertia and variability with adolescent depression and risk based on maternal history, controlling for average affect. Depressed adolescents showed more inert and diminished positive affect, and more variable and elevated negative affect compared to lower- and higher-risk youth, though associations attenuated after controlling for average affect. No differences were identified between never-depressed higher-risk and lower-risk youth. Additional longitudinal studies are needed to evaluate whether altered affect dynamics in daily life precede depression onset to understand their utility for developing preventive interventions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    暴露于人造光会影响人类的表现,这对于保持健康的工作和睡眠至关重要。然而,现有的研究还没有探索睡眠表现和人类状态之间的内在联系随着时间的推移在睡前光照干预(LEI).
    为了研究睡前光照改变的时间依赖性影响,选择四个LEI分组(#L1-#L4)和一个时间因子(D8,D9和D10)用于封闭空间中的睡眠实验。招募的48名年轻人可用于数据分析。主观警觉性(SA),负面影响(NA),主观睡眠,通过卡罗林斯卡嗜睡量表测量客观睡眠,正面和负面影响时间表,第二天自我评估睡眠质量,以及腕关节肌动描记术和睡眠日记的联合评估,分别。统计分析用于光暴露对人体状态(对应于SA和NA)和睡眠表现的影响,而过程模型帮助构建了两者之间的关联。
    统计效应表明,时间对主观睡眠和睡前警觉性的变化具有显着的主要影响;LEI仅对睡眠发作潜伏期(SOL)具有显着的主要影响。在接受改变的睡前光照后,在D9(p=0.022)和D10(p=0.044)的睡前时间,平均SA增加;对NA没有观察到显着影响;平均主观睡眠从D8到D10显着增加。此外,5个活动记法估计的睡眠参数是相互关联的。鉴于此,确定了一种链接的途径关系。SOL在睡前状态和客观睡眠之间起中介作用,通过主观睡眠与觉醒状态有关。
    我们的研究表明,睡前光照改变对睡眠表现的时间依赖性影响与睡前和觉醒时的人类状态有关,影响其对睡眠健康的预测。
    UNASSIGNED: Exposure to artificial light influences human performance, which is essential for maintaining healthy work and sleep. However, existing research has not explored the intrinsic links between sleep performance and human states over time under prebedtime light exposure interventions (LEIs).
    UNASSIGNED: To investigate the time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure, four LEI groupings (#L1 - #L4) and a Time factor (D8, D9, and D10) were chosen for sleep experiments in enclosed spaces. Forty-eight young adults recruited were available for data analysis. Subjective alertness (SA), negative affect (NA), subjective sleep, and objective sleep were measured via the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, Next-day Self-assessment Sleep Quality, and joint assessment of wrist actigraphy and sleep diaries, respectively. Statistical analysis was used for the effects of light exposure on the human states (corresponding to the SA and NA) and sleep performance, while the process model helped construct the associations between the two.
    UNASSIGNED: The statistical effects revealed that the Time had a significant main effect on subjective sleep and changes in prebedtime alertness; the LEI had a significant main effect only on sleep onset latency (SOL). After undergoing altered prebedtime light exposure, the mean SA increased at prebedtime of D9 (p = 0.022) and D10 (p = 0.044); No significant effect on the NA was observed; Mean subjective sleep had a significant increase from D8 to D10. Moreover, five actigraphy-estimated sleep parameters were interrelated. In light of this, a chained pathway relationship was identified. The SOL played a mediating predictor between prebedtime state and objective sleep, which was linked to the awakening state through subjective sleep.
    UNASSIGNED: Our study suggests that time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure on sleep performance are associated with human states at prebedtime and awakening, with implications for its prediction of sleep health.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目前尚不清楚成瘾神经临床评估标志物如何影响,寻求感觉,和执行功能-有助于物质使用发展。这项研究调查了消极影响和寻求感觉与物质使用的关联是否因执行功能而异。参与者是167名青少年(47%为女性),每年参加四年(Mage=14.07,SDage=0.54,时间1)。对于执行功能较低的青少年,较高的负面影响与较高的物质使用之间存在人内双向关联。无论执行功能水平如何,在14岁时寻求较高感觉的青少年从14岁到17岁表现出越来越多的物质使用轨迹。负面影响和物质使用在个体内部相互影响,而感觉寻求预测个体之间的物质使用。
    It is unknown how the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment markers-negative affect, sensation seeking, and executive function-contribute to substance use development. This study examined whether associations of negative affect and sensation seeking with substance use vary by executive function. Participants were 167 adolescents (47% female) who participated annually for four years (Mage = 14.07, SDage = 0.54 at Time 1). There were within-person bidirectional associations between higher negative affect and higher substance use for adolescents with lower executive function. Adolescents with higher sensation seeking at age 14 exhibited increasing substance use trajectories from age 14 to 17, regardless of executive function level. Negative affect and substance use influence each other within individuals, whereas sensation seeking predicts substance use between individuals.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:最近的一项荟萃分析显示,在月经周期的黄体期,迷走神经介导的心率变异性(vmHRV;情绪调节能力的生物标志物)显著降低。正如两项后续研究表明的那样,这些vmHRV下降主要是由黄体孕酮(P4)增加所致.然而,分析还显示,vmHRV对循环的反应性存在显著的个体差异,这与长期证据一致,表明个体间对周期的情绪敏感性存在差异。本研究开始调查vmHRV周期性的这些个体间差异是否可以解释谁出现经前情绪变化的风险较高。我们预计黄体中期vmHRV降低的程度会更大,从而预示着负面影响的经前增加。
    方法:我们对自然循环社区样本(N=31,M=26.03年)进行了观察性研究。在六个星期的时间里,参与者完成(a)负面情绪的每日评级和(b)在排卵中平衡实验室访问,黄体中期,和外围阶段。根据阳性排卵试验安排实验室访视,并包括基线vmHRV和唾液卵巢类固醇水平的评估。
    结果:与先前的研究一致,多水平模型表明,大多数样本显示排卵至黄体中期vmHRV降低,然而,与经前情绪变化无关。有趣的是,只有vmHRV黄体增加的亚组,其负面影响在月经前明显恶化,而在月经后改善。
    结论:本研究开始研究vmHRV的周期性变化,作为情绪对月经周期敏感性的潜在生物标志物。结果表明,这些关联的复杂性比最初预期的要高。鉴于vmHRV中只有非典型的黄体增加与更大的经前负面影响相关。讨论了潜在的潜在机制,其中黄体vmHRV可能会增加经前负面影响较大的患者调节情绪的指数补偿努力。然而,未来的研究应在这些发现的基础上,进一步探讨vmHRV周期性与经期相关情绪变化之间的关联.
    BACKGROUND: A recent meta-analysis revealed that vagally mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV; a biomarker of emotion regulation capacity) significantly decreases in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. As two follow-up studies suggest, these vmHRV decreases are driven primarily by increased luteal progesterone (P4). However, analyses also revealed significant interindividual differences in vmHRV reactivity to the cycle, which is in line with longstanding evidence for interindividual differences in mood sensitivity to the cycle. The present study begins to investigate whether these interindividual differences in vmHRV cyclicity can explain who is at higher risk of showing premenstrual emotional changes. We expected a greater degree of midluteal vmHRV decrease to be predictive of a greater premenstrual increase in negative affect.
    METHODS: We conducted an observational study with a naturally cycling community sample (N = 31, M = 26.03 years). Over a span of six weeks, participants completed (a) daily ratings of negative affect and (b) counterbalanced lab visits in their ovulatory, midluteal, and perimenstrual phases. Lab visits were scheduled based on positive ovulation tests and included assessments of baseline vmHRV and salivary ovarian steroid levels.
    RESULTS: In line with previous research, multilevel models suggest that most of the sample shows ovulatory-to-midluteal vmHRV decreases which, however, were not associated with premenstrual emotional changes. Interestingly, it was only the subgroup with luteal increases in vmHRV whose negative affect markedly worsened premenstrually and improved postmenstrually.
    CONCLUSIONS: The present study begins to investigate cyclical changes in vmHRV as a potential biomarker of mood sensitivity to the menstrual cycle. The results demonstrate a higher level of complexity in these associations than initially expected, given that only atypical midluteal increases in vmHRV are associated with greater premenstrual negative affect. Potential underlying mechanisms are discussed, among those the possibility that luteal vmHRV increases index compensatory efforts to regulate emotion in those with greater premenstrual negative affect. However, future studies with larger and clinical samples and more granular vmHRV assessments should build on these findings and further explore associations between vmHRV cyclicity and menstrually related mood changes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    忧虑倾向是一种诊断特征,可预测负面影响(NA)增加,可能有助于防止负面的情绪对比。尽管NA的离散类型随着唤醒的维度而变化,特质担忧预测高与高的程度日常生活中NA的低唤醒形式尚不清楚。这种区别对于概念化担忧如何干扰各种疾病中的适应性情绪具有重要意义。本研究(未预注册)旨在使用生态瞬时评估来分离特征担忧对日常生活中高(N=88)和低(N=122)唤醒NA的影响,同时控制潜在的生理和心理困惑。在基线时评估参与者的特质担忧和抑郁症状,然后报告他们的影响,心率,每天锻炼三次,持续一周。多水平模型显示,特质担忧在控制瞬时心率后预测高唤醒和低唤醒NA的增加,日常锻炼,和抑郁症。相比之下,基线抑郁症状只能预测日常生活中的低觉醒NA。研究结果支持对比避免的担忧模型,并表明担忧与日常生活中状态NA的增加有关,独立于唤醒。
    Worry proneness is a transdiagnostic trait that predicts increased negative affect (NA), potentially in the service of preventing negative emotional contrasts. Although discrete types of NA vary along the dimension of arousal, the extent to which trait worry predicts high vs. low arousal forms of NA in daily life is unclear. This distinction has important implications for conceptualising how worry may perturb adaptive emotionality in various disorders. The present study (not pre-registered) aimed to isolate the effects of trait worry on high (N = 88) and low (N = 122) arousal NA in daily life using ecological momentary assessment while controlling for potential physical and psychological confounds. Participants were assessed for trait worry and depressive symptoms at baseline then reported their affect, heart rate, and exercise three times per day for one week. Multilevel models revealed that trait worry predicted both increased high and low arousal NA after controlling for momentary heart rate, daily exercise, and depression. In contrast, baseline depressive symptoms only predicted low arousal NA in daily life. Findings support the contrast avoidance model of worry and suggest that worry is linked to increased state NA in daily life, independent of arousal.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    酒精使用障碍(AUD)是世界范围内日益严重的问题,对健康和经济造成了令人难以置信的负担。尽管AUD影响了所有背景和人口统计学的人,越来越多的证据表明,饮酒模式存在明显的性别差异,以及AUD诱导的负面情绪或过度激动.有问题的饮酒率在女性中显著上升,女性在禁欲中面临更严重的负面情绪后果,如焦虑或情绪障碍合并症的风险增加,以及更严重的抑郁症状。因此,近年来,大量使用或有方法饮酒(乙醇)的临床前文献已经积累,以更好地理解性别是饮酒和禁欲引起的负面情绪的生物学变量.小鼠被广泛用来模拟饮酒,因为它们有利于基因操纵策略,许多菌株会自愿饮酒。这些小鼠研究的性别特异性结果,然而,一直不一致。因此,这篇综述旨在总结目前关于AUD相关的偶然饮酒和禁欲诱导的小鼠负面情绪的性别差异的知识。引入了各种偶然的小鼠饮酒模型和基于负面情绪的行为范式,随后在性别差异的背景下进行了讨论,以显示AUD小鼠临床前研究中性别特异性的迹象越来越多。通过这次审查,我们希望为未来AUD临床前小鼠模型中潜在性别差异的研究提供信息,并提供越来越多的证据支持需要在未来的研究中更广泛地纳入临床前女性受试者.
    Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) is a growing problem worldwide, causing an incredible burden on health and the economy. Though AUD impacts people of all backgrounds and demographics, increasing evidence has suggested robust sex differences in alcohol drinking patterns and AUD-induced negative emotionality or hyperkatifeia. Rates of problematic drinking have significantly risen among women, and women face more severe negative emotional consequences in abstinence such as increased risk of comorbidity with an anxiety or mood disorder and more severe symptoms of depression. As such, a bevy of preclinical literature using contingent methods of alcohol (ethanol) consumption has amassed in recent years to better understand sex as a biological variable in alcohol drinking and abstinence-induced negative emotionality. Mice are widely used to model alcohol drinking, as they are conducive to genetic manipulation strategies, and many strains will voluntarily consume alcohol. Sex-specific results from these mouse studies, however, have been inconsistent. Therefore, this review aims to summarize the current knowledge on sex differences in AUD-related contingent ethanol drinking and abstinence-induced negative emotionality in mice. Various contingent mouse drinking models and negative emotional-based behavioral paradigms are introduced and subsequently discussed in the context of sex differences to show increasing indications of sex specificity in mouse preclinical studies of AUD. With this review, we hope to inform future research on potential sex differences in preclinical mouse models of AUD and provide mounting evidence supporting the need for more widespread inclusion of preclinical female subjects in future studies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    基于实验室的研究表明,对各种压力源的反应引起的心理压力会引发心血管系统的急性变化。需要更好地了解自然环境中心理压力与心血管反应之间的时间关联的新证据。这项研究检查了日常生活中心理压力和心率变异性(HRV)的关系,以高分辨率超过2周,考虑到身体活动的影响。参与者(n=34)每天完成6次生态瞬时评估(EMA),报告感知压力水平,低唤醒负面影响(LNA),和高唤醒的负面影响。佩戴胸部安装的心率监测器以评估HRV。使用多水平模型来检查心理压力水平与先前/随后的HRV之间的关联。在前一小时内减少的时域HRV测量值(R波到R波间隔的平均值和标准偏差)预测了更高的感知压力水平。频域HRV测量较高的低频到高频(LF/HF)和较低的HF与总功率(HFnu)的比率,在前10分钟预测较高的感知压力水平,表明交感神经系统活动占主导地位。在接下来的10分钟内,EMA报告较高的感知压力水平与时域HRV测量值降低有关。另一方面,较高的LNA与HRV测量增加有关,例如在接下来的一小时内更低的LF/HF和更高的HFnu。观察到的动态关联可能对日常压力和心血管健康管理中的“及时”干预措施具有治疗意义。
    Laboratory-based studies have shown that psychological stress caused by response to various stressors triggers acute changes in the cardiovascular system. A better understanding is needed of the emerging evidence on temporal associations between psychological stress and cardiovascular responses in natural settings. This study examined the association of psychological stress and heart rate variability (HRV) in daily life, at high resolution over 2 weeks, taking the effect of physical activity into account. Participants (n = 34) completed ecological momentary assessments (EMA) 6 times per day, reporting levels of perceived stress, low-arousal negative affect (LNA), and high-arousal negative affect. Chest-mounted heart-rate monitors were worn to assess HRV. Multilevel models were used to examine the association between psychological stress levels and preceding/subsequent HRV. Reduced time domain HRV measures (mean and standard deviation of R-wave to R-wave intervals) during the prior hour predicted higher levels of perceived stress. Frequency domain HRV measures higher low to high frequency (LF/HF) and lower HF to total power (HF nu) ratios during the preceding 10 min predicted higher perceived stress levels, suggesting the dominance of sympathetic nervous system activity. EMA reports of higher perceived stress levels were associated with reduced time domain HRV measures during the following 10 min. On the other hand, higher LNA were related to increased HRV measures, such as lower LF/HF and higher HF nu during the following hour. The dynamic associations observed may have therapeutic implications for \'just-in-time\' interventions in the management of daily stress and cardiovascular health.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究考察了双因素影响模型在多大程度上解释了护理人员所经历的护理评估如何影响其自身的幸福感。我们使用来自国家护理研究(NSOC)的三波数据进行了具有时变预测因子的潜在生长曲线模型,以研究人与人之间(BP)和人内(WP)护理评估对正面和负面影响的影响。此外,我们同时用多层次建模对WP在活动参与和情感体验方面的差异进行了建模。然后,我们测试了活动参与在WP护理评估与情绪效价之间的关系中的调节作用。我们发现,BP和WP护理负面评价也有助于照顾者的积极影响,类似于消极影响。负面评价和情绪效价的时变效应与双因素模型一致。未来的纵向调查可以针对WP和BP活动参与,以减轻护理人员的护理认知评估。
    The present study examines the extent to which a two-factor model of affect explains how caregiving appraisals experienced by caregivers influence their own well-being. We used data from three waves of Nation Study of Caregiving (NSOC) to conduct latent growth curve models with the time-varying predictors to investigate the effect of between-person (BP) and within-person (WP) caregiving appraisals on positive and negative affect. Furthermore, we simultaneously modeled WP differences in activity participation and affective experience with multilevel modeling. Then, we tested the moderating effect of activity participation in the association between WP caregiving appraisals and emotional valence. We found that BP and WP caregiving negative appraisal also contribute to caregiver positive affect similar to that of negative affect. Time-varying effects of negative appraisals and emotional valence are consistent with the two-factor model. Future longitudinal investigations could target WP and BP activity participation to alleviate caregiving cognitive appraisal among caregivers.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:通常通过自我报告来评估情绪清晰度,但是也做出了被动测量的努力,它具有优势,例如避免了由于社会期望偏见或对情绪清晰度的洞察力不足而导致的潜在反应不准确。在生态瞬时评估(EMA)中对情绪项目的响应时间(RT)可能是情绪清晰度的间接指标。另一个建议的指标是漂移率参数,它假设,除了一个人对情感项目的反应有多快,情绪清晰度的测量还需要考虑参与者在提供回答时的谨慎程度。
    目的:本文旨在研究来自EMA情绪项目的RTs和漂移率参数作为情绪清晰度个体差异指标的信度和效度。
    方法:次要数据分析是对196名1型糖尿病成年人的数据进行的,他们完成了为期2周的EMA研究,每天完成5至6次调查。如果较低的RTs和较高的漂移率(来自EMA情绪项目)是情绪清晰度的指标,我们假设更高的水平(即,更高的清晰度)应该与更高的生活满意度相关;较低的神经质水平,抑郁症,焦虑,和糖尿病困扰;情绪调节困难更少。因为先前的文献表明情绪清晰度可能是特定的,分别检查了负面影响(NA)和正面影响的EMA项目。
    结果:在少量EMA提示的情况下,所提出的情绪清晰度指标的可靠性是可以接受的(即,总共4到7个提示或1到2天的EMA调查)。与期望一致,多个EMA中NA项目的平均漂移率预期与其他指标有关联,如r=-0.27(P<.001)与抑郁症状的相关性,r=-0.27(P=.001)伴有焦虑症状,r=-0.15(P=0.03),情绪调节困难,r=0.63(P<.001),RT到NA项目。NA漂移率更高的人对NA情绪项目的反应更快,有更大的主观幸福感(例如,抑郁症状更少),并且在整体情绪调节方面的困难较少,这些都与对情感清晰度度量的期望一致。与预期相反,平均RT与NA项目的效价,积极影响项目的漂移率,我们的结果没有强烈支持积极影响项目的RT。
    结论:研究结果为NA漂移率作为情绪清晰度指标的有效性提供了初步支持,但没有为其他基于RT的清晰度指标提供支持。证据是初步的,因为样本量不足以检测小但潜在有意义的相关性,因为糖尿病EMA研究的样本量被选择用于其他更主要的研究问题。需要进一步研究被动情绪清晰度措施。
    BACKGROUND: Emotional clarity has often been assessed with self-report measures, but efforts have also been made to measure it passively, which has advantages such as avoiding potential inaccuracy in responses stemming from social desirability bias or poor insight into emotional clarity. Response times (RTs) to emotion items administered in ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) may be an indirect indicator of emotional clarity. Another proposed indicator is the drift rate parameter, which assumes that, aside from how fast a person responds to emotion items, the measurement of emotional clarity also requires the consideration of how careful participants were in providing responses.
    OBJECTIVE: This paper aims to examine the reliability and validity of RTs and drift rate parameters from EMA emotion items as indicators of individual differences in emotional clarity.
    METHODS: Secondary data analysis was conducted on data from 196 adults with type 1 diabetes who completed a 2-week EMA study involving the completion of 5 to 6 surveys daily. If lower RTs and higher drift rates (from EMA emotion items) were indicators of emotional clarity, we hypothesized that greater levels (ie, higher clarity) should be associated with greater life satisfaction; lower levels of neuroticism, depression, anxiety, and diabetes distress; and fewer difficulties with emotion regulation. Because prior literature suggested emotional clarity could be valence specific, EMA items for negative affect (NA) and positive affect were examined separately.
    RESULTS: Reliability of the proposed indicators of emotional clarity was acceptable with a small number of EMA prompts (ie, 4 to 7 prompts in total or 1 to 2 days of EMA surveys). Consistent with expectations, the average drift rate of NA items across multiple EMAs had expected associations with other measures, such as correlations of r=-0.27 (P<.001) with depression symptoms, r=-0.27 (P=.001) with anxiety symptoms, r=-0.15 (P=.03) with emotion regulation difficulties, and r=0.63 (P<.001) with RTs to NA items. People with a higher NA drift rate responded faster to NA emotion items, had greater subjective well-being (eg, fewer depression symptoms), and had fewer difficulties with overall emotion regulation, which are all aligned with the expectation for an emotional clarity measure. Contrary to expectations, the validities of average RTs to NA items, the drift rate of positive affect items, and RTs to positive affect items were not strongly supported by our results.
    CONCLUSIONS: Study findings provided initial support for the validity of NA drift rate as an indicator of emotional clarity but not for that of other RT-based clarity measures. Evidence was preliminary because the sample size was not sufficient to detect small but potentially meaningful correlations, as the sample size of the diabetes EMA study was chosen for other more primary research questions. Further research on passive emotional clarity measures is needed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号