N250

N250
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    两个经典的实验范式——掩蔽重复启动和边界范式——在理解视觉单词识别过程中发挥了关键作用。传统上,这些范式已被不同的研究人员社区采用,有着自己悠久的研究传统。然而,对文献的回顾表明,与两种范式建立的文字处理的脑电相关可能显示出有趣的相似性,特别是在位置方面,定时,以及N1和N250效应的方向。然而,到目前为止,这两种范式之间没有进行直接比较。在目前的研究中,我们使用组合的眼动追踪/脑电图,使用相同的材料(单个汉字)作为刺激进行这种受试者内比较.为了便于直接比较,我们使用了简化版本的边界范式-单词边界范式。我们的结果显示了N1和N250对两种范例的典型早期重复效应。然而,N250中的重复效应(即,与不同单词的素数/预览相比,相同单词的素数/预览后的消极性降低)在单个单词边界范式下比在蒙面启动下更大。对于N1效应,两种范式的重复效应相似,与交替相比,重复后显示更大的N1。因此,结果表明,在神经水平,一个简短的呈现和蒙面的中央凹主要产生质量上类似的促进效果,在视觉单词识别作为一个旁凹预览在一个单一的扫视前,尽管在后一种情况下,这种影响似乎更强。
    Two classic experimental paradigms - masked repetition priming and the boundary paradigm - have played a pivotal role in understanding the process of visual word recognition. Traditionally, these paradigms have been employed by different communities of researchers, with their own long-standing research traditions. Nevertheless, a review of the literature suggests that the brain-electric correlates of word processing established with both paradigms may show interesting similarities, in particular with regard to the location, timing, and direction of N1 and N250 effects. However, as of yet, no direct comparison has been undertaken between the two paradigms. In the current study, we used combined eye-tracking/EEG to perform such a within-subject comparison using the same materials (single Chinese characters) as stimuli. To facilitate direct comparisons, we used a simplified version of the boundary paradigm - the single word boundary paradigm. Our results show the typical early repetition effects of N1 and N250 for both paradigms. However, repetition effects in N250 (i.e., a reduced negativity following identical-word primes/previews as compared to different-word primes/previews) were larger with the single word boundary paradigm than with masked priming. For N1 effects, repetition effects were similar across the two paradigms, showing a larger N1 after repetitions as compared to alternations. Therefore, the results indicate that at the neural level, a briefly presented and masked foveal prime produces qualitatively similar facilitatory effects on visual word recognition as a parafoveal preview before a single saccade, although such effects appear to be stronger in the latter case.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    格兰杰等人。(2006年)是第一个使用ERP蒙面启动来探索语音和拼字表示对视觉文字处理的不同贡献的人。在这里,我们调整了他们的范式来研究聋哑读者的文字处理。我们调查了阅读匹配的聋人和听力读者(n=36)在文字处理期间是否表现出与正字法和语音代码激活相关的不同ERP效果。在视觉掩蔽启动范例中,参与者执行了“走/不走”分类任务(检测偶尔的动物单词).关键目标单词之前是正字相关(转置字母-TL)或语音相关(伪同音-PH)掩蔽的非单词原语,与相同的目标单词之前是字母替换(对照)非单词原语。听力阅读器表现出典型的N250和N400启动效应(与TL或PH启动目标相比,对照的负电性更大),TL和PH启动效应没有差异。对于聋人读者来说,N250PH启动效应较晚(250-350ms),在这个时间窗口中,他们对TL素数表现出逆转的N250启动效应。N400TL和PH引发效应在组间没有差异。对于听众来说,具有更好的语音和拼写技能的人显示出更大的早期N250PH和TL启动效果(150-250ms)。对于聋人读者来说,具有更好语音技能的人在N250后期窗口中表现出更大的反向TL启动效应。我们推测,语音知识可以调节聋人读者对整个单词正字法表示和/或从亚词汇到词汇表示的映射的依赖程度。
    Grainger et al. (2006) were the first to use ERP masked priming to explore the differing contributions of phonological and orthographic representations to visual word processing. Here we adapted their paradigm to examine word processing in deaf readers. We investigated whether reading-matched deaf and hearing readers (n = 36) exhibit different ERP effects associated with the activation of orthographic and phonological codes during word processing. In a visual masked priming paradigm, participants performed a go/no-go categorization task (detect an occasional animal word). Critical target words were preceded by orthographically-related (transposed letter - TL) or phonologically-related (pseudohomophone - PH) masked non-word primes were contrasted with the same target words preceded by letter substitution (control) non-words primes. Hearing readers exhibited typical N250 and N400 priming effects (greater negativity for control compared to TL or PH primed targets), and the TL and PH priming effects did not differ. For deaf readers, the N250 PH priming effect was later (250-350 ms), and they showed a reversed N250 priming effect for TL primes in this time window. The N400 TL and PH priming effects did not differ between groups. For hearing readers, those with better phonological and spelling skills showed larger early N250 PH and TL priming effects (150-250 ms). For deaf readers, those with better phonological skills showed a larger reversed TL priming effect in the late N250 window. We speculate that phonological knowledge modulates how strongly deaf readers rely on whole-word orthographic representations and/or the mapping from sublexical to lexical representations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的实验表明,与以前不熟悉的人的短暂相遇会导致建立新的面部表征,它可以通过新学习的面部的完全新颖的图片来激活。本研究检查了这种新颖的神经表征随着时间的推移有多稳定,and,具体来说,他们是如何在学习后的第一个24小时内巩固的。在参与者之间的设计中使用事件相关的脑电位(ERP),我们证明,在学习后立即在枕部-颞部N250中明显的面部熟悉效应是明显的。然后这些影响发生变化,在初次相遇后4小时,几乎完全没有与熟悉程度相关的ERP差异。严重的,学习后24小时,最初的熟悉效应重新出现。这些发现表明,新面部表征的神经相关性随着时间的推移并不稳定,而是在学习后的第一天发生变化。由此产生的变化模式与合并过程是一致的。
    Previous experiments have shown that a brief encounter with a previously unfamiliar person leads to the establishment of new facial representations, which can be activated by completely novel pictures of the newly learnt face. The present study examined how stable such novel neural representations are over time, and, specifically, how they become consolidated within the first 24 h after learning. Using event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in a between-participants design, we demonstrate that clear face familiarity effects in the occipito-temporal N250 are evident immediately after learning. These effects then undergo change, with a nearly complete absence of familiarity-related ERP differences 4 h after the initial encounter. Critically, 24 h after learning, the original familiarity effect re-emerges. These findings suggest that the neural correlates of novel face representations are not stable over time but change during the first day after learning. The resulting pattern of change is consistent with a process of consolidation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    面部记忆能力的个体差异已被证明与大脑活动的个体差异有关。本研究调查了事件相关脑电位中N250成分的脑行为关系,这被认为是面部熟悉的神经标志。我们使用了一个指定的任务,在一次会议的多次演示中,必须区分典型的目标面孔和几个(高和低特征)非目标面孔。分别,面部记忆/识别能力是通过简单和困难的任务来测量的。我们在整个会话期间将N250振幅的增加复制到目标面,并观察到非目标面的类似增加,指示这些面部的内存表示的积累。在个人层面上,更大的跨会话N250幅度增加到低区别性非目标面部与更快的面部识别相关,如在简单的任务中测量的。这些发现表明,将非目标人脸非故意编码到内存中与快速识别明确学习的人脸有关;也就是说,偶然和有意的面部记忆存在共同的差异。
    Individual differences in face memory abilities have been shown to be related to individual differences in brain activity. The present study investigated brain-behavior relationships for the N250 component in event-related brain potentials, which is taken as a neural sign of face familiarity. We used a task in which a designated, typical target face and several (high- and low-distinctive) nontarget faces had to be distinguished during multiple presentations across a session. Separately, face memory/recognition abilities were measured with easy versus difficult tasks. We replicated an increase of the N250 amplitude to the target face across the session and observed a similar increase for the non-target faces, indicating the build-up of memory representations also for these faces. On the interindividual level, larger across-session N250 amplitude increases to low-distinctive non-target faces were related to faster face recognition as measured in an easy task. These findings indicate that non-intentional encoding of non-target faces into memory is associated with the swift recognition of explicitly learned faces; that is, there is shared variance of incidental and intentional face memory.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Valentine的有影响力的基于范数的多维人脸空间模型(nMDFS)预测,感知的独特性随着与范数的距离而增加。最近已显示出枕骨-时间事件相关电位(ERP)对距离-范数(P200)或熟悉度(N250,晚期负性)的变化有选择性地做出反应,分别(Wuttke&Schweinberger,2019)。尽管越来越多的证据表明,行为层面的面部感知技能存在个体差异,很少有研究关注它们的电生理相关性。为了揭示人脸空间中潜在的个体差异,我们对比了高和低表演者在人脸识别方面的距离-正常(P200)和熟悉度(N250)。我们复制了P200与规范的距离和N250熟悉度效应。重要的是,我们观察到:i)与面部识别的高性能者相比,响应较低,特别是在P200中较小的距离到规范的影响方面,可能表明与高表演者相比,低的“扩展”面部空间较少;ii)增加了N250对高表演者熟悉的原始面孔的响应,建议更健壮的面部身份表征。总之,这些发现表明,早期基于规范的面部编码和鲁棒的面部表示对个人面部识别技能的贡献,并表明ERPs可以提供一种有希望的途径来理解面部感知及其神经认知相关的个体差异。
    Valentine\'s influential norm-based multidimensional face-space model (nMDFS) predicts that perceived distinctiveness of a face increases with its distance to the norm. Occipito-temporal event-related potentials (ERPs) have been recently shown to respond selectively to variations in distance-to-norm (P200) or familiarity (N250, late negativity), respectively (Wuttke & Schweinberger, 2019). Despite growing evidence on interindividual differences in face perception skills at the behavioral level, little research has focused on their electrophysiological correlates. To reveal potential interindividual differences in face spaces, we contrasted high and low performers in face recognition in regards to distance-to-norm (P200) and familiarity (N250). We replicated both the P200 distance-to-norm and the N250 familiarity effect. Importantly, we observed: i) reduced responses in low compared to high performers of face recognition, especially in terms of smaller distance-to-norm effects in the P200, possibly indicating less \'expanded\' face spaces in low compared to high performers; ii) increased N250 responses to familiar original faces in high performers, suggesting more robust face identity representations. In summary, these findings suggest the contribution of both early norm-based face coding and robust face representations to individual face recognition skills, and indicate that ERPs can offer a promising route to understand individual differences in face perception and their neurocognitive correlates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究旨在探索熟悉度如何在不同条件下调节面部的神经处理:直立或倒置,中性或情绪化。为此,32名参与者(25名女性;年龄:M=27.7岁,SD=9.3)在EEG记录期间执行了两个面部/情绪识别任务。在第一个任务中,为了研究面部处理,在目标检测任务中呈现了三种不同类别的面部刺激:著名的熟悉面孔,亲人的面孔,陌生的面孔根据每个熟悉程度探索人脸倒置效果,这些面部刺激也是颠倒的。在第二个任务中,研究情绪面部处理,对个人熟悉和不熟悉的面孔进行了情感识别任务。行为结果表明,随着面部熟悉度的增加,在识别面部情绪表情方面的表现有所改善,与以前的文献一致。关于电生理结果,我们发现倒置的P100,N170和N250的振幅比直立的脸增加,独立于他们的熟悉程度。此外,我们在P100和N170时间窗口没有发现熟悉效应,但是我们发现,与不熟悉的面孔相比,个人熟悉的N250振幅更大。此结果支持以下推理:在N250时间窗口中,面部熟悉度增加了神经活动,这可以解释为通过观看我们所爱的人的面孔而提示的额外信息的处理,与不熟悉的人相比。
    The present study aims to explore how familiarity modulates the neural processing of faces under different conditions: upright or inverted, neutral or emotional. To this purpose, 32 participants (25 female; age: M = 27.7 years, SD = 9.3) performed two face/emotion identification tasks during EEG recording. In the first task, to study facial processing, three different categories of facial stimuli were presented during a target detection task: famous familiar faces, faces of loved ones, and unfamiliar faces. To explore the face inversion effect according to each level of familiarity, these facial stimuli were also presented upside down. In the second task, to study emotional face processing, an emotional identification task on personally familiar and unfamiliar faces was conducted. The behavioural results showed an improved performance in the identification of facial expressions of emotion with the increase of facial familiarity, consistent with the previous literature. Regarding electrophysiological results, we found increased amplitudes of the P100, N170, and N250 for inverted compared to upright faces, independently of their degree of familiarity. Moreover, we did not find familiarity effects at the P100 and N170 time-windows, but we found that N250 amplitude was larger for personally familiar compared to unfamiliar faces. This result supports the reasoning that the facial familiarity increases the neural activity during the N250 time-window, which may be explained by the processing of additional information prompted by the viewing of our loved ones faces, in contrast to what happens with unfamiliar individuals.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    众所周知,熟悉和不熟悉的面孔被不同地处理,但令人惊讶的是,人们对熟悉度是如何随着时间的推移而建立起来的,以及新奇面孔是如何逐渐在大脑中表现出来的,人们对此知之甚少。这里,我们使用事件相关脑电位(ERPs)在预先登记的,纵向研究,以检查在认识一个人的前八个月中伴随面部和身份学习的神经过程。具体来说,我们研究了增加现实生活中的熟悉度如何影响视觉识别(N250熟悉度效应)和人相关知识的整合(持续熟悉度效应,SFE)。16名一年级本科生分三次接受了测试,大约一,五,在学年开始八个月后,他们在大学里遇到的一个新朋友和一个陌生的人的高度可变的“环境”图像。熟悉一个月后,我们观察到新朋友的ERP熟悉效果。虽然在研究过程中N250效应有所增加,没有观察到SFE的变化。这些结果表明,相对于身份特定知识的整合,视觉人脸表示的发展更快。
    It is well-established that familiar and unfamiliar faces are processed differently, but surprisingly little is known about how familiarity builds up over time and how novel faces gradually become represented in the brain. Here, we used event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in a pre-registered, longitudinal study to examine the neural processes accompanying face and identity learning during the first eight months of knowing a person. Specifically, we examined how increasing real-life familiarity affects visual recognition (N250 Familiarity Effect) and the integration of person-related knowledge (Sustained Familiarity Effect, SFE). Sixteen first-year undergraduates were tested in three sessions, approximately one, five, and eight months after the start of the academic year, with highly variable \"ambient\" images of a new friend they had met at university and of an unfamiliar person. We observed clear ERP familiarity effects for the new friend after one month of familiarity. While there was an increase in the N250 effect over the course of the study, no change in the SFE was observed. These results suggest that visual face representations develop faster relative to the integration of identity-specific knowledge.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    从声音中识别人可能会被声音的独特性所促进,以类似于面部报道的方式。然而,人们对语音学习的神经时间过程以及面部信息在语音学习中的作用知之甚少。基于视听整合在识别熟悉的人中的证据,我们研究了与独特或非独特面孔相关的语音学习的行为和电生理相关性。我们重复了十二个不熟悉的声音,说出简短的句子,在六个学习测试周期中,以及独特或不独特的面孔(在语音演示之前和期间描绘)。在学习过程中,相对于非独特面孔,独特面孔增加了早期视觉诱发(N170,P200,N250)潜力,并且面部独特调制的语音在枕部颞部和额中央电极处引起缓慢的EEG活动。在测试中,以前用独特面孔学习的单峰呈现的声音比用非独特面孔学习的声音更快地分类,也比小说的声音更快。此外,以前用面部学习的声音会引起类似N250的成分,该成分在地形上与通常观察到的面部刺激相似。这种语音诱导的N250的初步来源定位与梭状回的来源兼容。一起来看,我们的研究结果为学习和语音识别过程中语音和面部处理区域之间的早期交互理论提供了支持.
    Recognizing people from their voices may be facilitated by a voice\'s distinctiveness, in a manner similar to that which has been reported for faces. However, little is known about the neural time-course of voice learning and the role of facial information in voice learning. Based on evidence for audiovisual integration in the recognition of familiar people, we studied the behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of voice learning associated with distinctive or non-distinctive faces. We repeated twelve unfamiliar voices uttering short sentences, together with either distinctive or non-distinctive faces (depicted before and during voice presentation) in six learning-test cycles. During learning, distinctive faces increased early visually-evoked (N170, P200, N250) potentials relative to non-distinctive faces, and face distinctiveness modulated voice-elicited slow EEG activity at the occipito-temporal and fronto-central electrodes. At the test, unimodally-presented voices previously learned with distinctive faces were classified more quickly than were voices learned with non-distinctive faces, and also more quickly than novel voices. Moreover, voices previously learned with faces elicited an N250-like component that was similar in topography to that typically observed for facial stimuli. The preliminary source localization of this voice-induced N250 was compatible with a source in the fusiform gyrus. Taken together, our findings provide support for a theory of early interaction between voice and face processing areas during both learning and voice recognition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在单个实验会话中学习的面孔在事件相关的脑电位(ERP)中引起熟悉效应,相对于N250组件中的陌生面孔,新学习的负幅度更大。然而,没有ERP研究在短暂的现实生活中接触后检查面部学习,目前尚不清楚在这种生态更有效的条件下学习新面孔需要多长时间。为了调查这些问题,本研究检查了是否鲁棒的图像独立表示,正如N250熟悉效应所反映的那样,可以通过分析新学习的身份和不熟悉的人的高度可变的图像引起的ERP,在短暂的不受约束的社交互动之后建立。在30分钟(实验1)和10分钟(实验2)相遇后观察到显着的N250熟悉效应,并在学习5分钟后观察到趋势(实验3),证明5-10分钟的曝光对于最初建立与图像无关的表示是足够的。此外,10分钟和30分钟后报告的影响程度相当,这表明社交相遇的前10分钟可能是至关重要的,从相同的遭遇额外的20分钟,不会为健壮的面部表示的初始形成增加进一步的好处。
    Faces learnt in a single experimental session elicit a familiarity effect in event-related brain potentials (ERPs), with more negative amplitudes for newly learnt relative to unfamiliar faces in the N250 component. However, no ERP study has examined face learning following a brief real-life encounter, and it is not clear how long it takes to learn new faces in such ecologically more valid conditions. To investigate these questions, the present study examined whether robust image-independent representations, as reflected in the N250 familiarity effect, could be established after a brief unconstrained social interaction by analysing the ERPs elicited by highly variable images of the newly learnt identity and an unfamiliar person. Significant N250 familiarity effects were observed after a 30-min (Experiment 1) and a 10-min (Experiment 2) encounter, and a trend was observed after 5 min of learning (Experiment 3), demonstrating that 5-10 min of exposure were sufficient for the initial establishment of image-independent representations. Additionally, the magnitude of the effects reported after 10 and 30 min was comparable suggesting that the first 10 min of a social encounter might be crucial, with extra 20 min from the same encounter not adding further benefit for the initial formation of robust face representations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当前的研究检查了听到个人级别名称的程度(例如,Jimmy)和类别级别的标签(例如,Hitchel)与新颖物体配对会在短暂(6分钟)的学习期内影响神经反应。记录了事件相关电位(ERP),而成年参与者(n=44)观看和听到了两种不同种类的命名新物体的范例。对每个标记条件的ERP进行检查,并在学习试验的前半部分和后半部分进行比较(~3分钟/半)。从试验的第一到后半部分,P1的平均振幅降低,N170的平均振幅增加。P1的减少是正确的。此外,右侧枕颞区的P1振幅大于左侧枕颞区,但仅适用于与单个级别标签配对的对象。类别级别标签未显示区域P1差异。N250成分在右枕颞区最大,并且在下半年的试验中,相对于类别级别名称,用个人级别标记的对象得到了增强。总的来说,这些发现强调了在成年人的短时间训练中标签依赖性视觉处理的展开.结果表明,语言标签具有重要的,自上而下的影响,在视觉处理上,标签特异性在6分钟的学习期内塑造了视觉皮层反应。
    The current study examines the extent to which hearing individual-level names (e.g., Jimmy) and category-level labels (e.g., Hitchel) paired with novel objects impacts neural responses across a brief (6 min) learning period. Event-related Potentials (ERPs) were recorded while adult participants (n = 44) viewed and heard exemplars of two different species of named novel objects. ERPs were examined for each labeling condition and compared across the first and second half of the learning trials (∼3 min/half). Mean amplitude decreased for the P1 and increased for the N170 from the first to the second half of trials. The decrease in P1 was right lateralized. In addition, the P1 amplitude recorded over right occipitotemporal regions was greater than left occipitotemporal areas, but only for objects paired with individual-level labels. Category-level labels did not show regional P1 differences. The N250 component was greatest over the right occipitotemporal region and was enhanced for objects labeled with individual-level relative to category-level names during the second half of trials. Overall, these findings highlight the unfolding of label-dependent visual processing across a short training period in adults. The results suggest that linguistic labels have an important, top-down impact, on visual processing and that label specificity shapes visuo-cortical responses within a 6-min learning period.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号