Motion perception

运动感知
  • 文章类型: Letter
    暂无摘要。
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    前庭运动感随着年龄的增长而下降,增加了大幅下跌的风险。我们在健康的老年人(n=40,年龄70-88岁)的6自由度运动平台上,使用自我运动方向辨别任务(每人2800个训练试验)进行了为期两周的感知学习干预。线性耳间和角度侧倾倾斜前庭阈值随着训练而提高(前/后差异的95%可信间隔),表明训练后敏感性改变。此外,改善感知能力转移到实际姿势(减少摇摆)和步态参数。被动自我动作辨别训练提供了一种新的有希望的方法来抵消与年龄相关的感觉下降。它可以降低跌倒的风险,从而保持个人的自主性和生活质量。
    Vestibular motion perception declines with age, increasing the risk of falling substantially. We performed a two-week perceptual learning intervention using a self-motion direction discrimination task (2800 training trials per person) on a 6 degrees of freedom motion platform in healthy older adults (n = 40, aged 70-88 yr). Linear inter-aural and angular roll tilt vestibular thresholds improved with training (95% credible interval for pre/post difference), suggesting altered sensitivity post-training. Moreover, improved perceptual abilities transfer to actual posture (reduced sway) and gait parameters. Passive self-motion discrimination training provides a new and promising way to counteract age-related sensory decline. It can reduce the risk of falling, and thereby maintain individual autonomy and quality of life.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    重度抑郁症(MDD)的特征是精神运动发育迟缓,其潜在的神经来源尚不清楚。精神运动迟缓可能与运动来源如运动皮层有关,或者,或者,在运动区域之外有神经变化的精神运动源,像视觉皮层这样的输入区域。研究了使用7特斯拉MRI的主要(n=41)和复制(n=18)MDD样品中的这两个替代假设。分析初级运动皮层(BA4)的全局和局部连通性,运动网络和颞叶中视皮层复合体(MT+),MDD的主要发现是:1)电机区域的局部和全局同步减少,局部到全局的输出增加,与精神运动迟缓无关,不过。2)减少的局部到局部BA4-MT功能连接(FC),这与精神运动迟缓有关。3)MT中与精神运动迟缓有关的全局同步降低和局部到全局输出增加。4)与精神运动迟缓有关的基于MT的运动知觉的心理物理测量中的变异性降低。一起,研究表明,视皮层MT及其与运动皮层的关系在介导精神运动发育迟缓中起关键作用。这支持关于MDD中精神运动迟缓的神经来源的精神运动过度运动假说。
    Major depressive disorder (MDD) is characterized by psychomotor retardation whose underlying neural source remains unclear. Psychomotor retardation may either be related to a motor source like the motor cortex or, alternatively, to a psychomotor source with neural changes outside motor regions, like input regions such as visual cortex. These two alternative hypotheses in main (n = 41) and replication (n = 18) MDD samples using 7 Tesla MRI are investigated. Analyzing both global and local connectivity in primary motor cortex (BA4), motor network and middle temporal visual cortex complex (MT+), the main findings in MDD are: 1) Reduced local and global synchronization and increased local-to-global output in motor regions, which do not correlate with psychomotor retardation, though. 2) Reduced local-to-local BA4 - MT+ functional connectivity (FC) which correlates with psychomotor retardation. 3) Reduced global synchronization and increased local-to-global output in MT+ which relate to psychomotor retardation. 4) Reduced variability in the psychophysical measures of MT+ based motion perception which relates to psychomotor retardation. Together, it is shown that visual cortex MT+ and its relation to motor cortex play a key role in mediating psychomotor retardation. This supports psychomotor over motor hypothesis about the neural source of psychomotor retardation in MDD.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在本文中,我们证明了我们之前提出的模型来解释视差聚散度眼动(视差聚散度响应,或DVR)响应白噪声视觉刺激的水平和垂直视差步骤,也可以很好地描述视觉运动领域对宽带刺激的短潜伏期眼部跟随反应(OFR)。此外,我们重新分析了数据,并将该模型应用于使用正弦波光栅(单个或两个或三个光栅的组合)和白噪声刺激的几项早期研究。该模型很好地说明了所有这些数据。该模型假设,短暂的眼球运动-OFR和DVR-可以由两个因素的操作来解释:兴奋性驱动,由刺激傅立叶分量的贡献的加权和确定,通过全局对比度归一化机制进行缩放。然后,通过刺激的总对比度对这两个因素的操作输出进行非线性缩放。尽管视差(水平和垂直)和运动信号在视觉场景分析中的作用不同,这些不同信号的最早处理阶段似乎非常相似。
    In this paper, we show that the model we proposed earlier to account for the disparity vergence eye movements (disparity vergence responses, or DVRs) in response to horizontal and vertical disparity steps of white noise visual stimuli also provides an excellent description of the short-latency ocular following responses (OFRs) to broadband stimuli in the visual motion domain. In addition, we reanalyzed the data and applied the model to several earlier studies that used sine-wave gratings (single or a combination of two or three gratings) and white noise stimuli. The model provides a very good account of all of these data. The model postulates that the short-latency eye movements-OFRs and DVRs-can be accounted for by the operation of two factors: an excitatory drive, determined by a weighted sum of contributions of stimulus Fourier components, scaled by a global contrast normalization mechanism. The output of the operation of these two factors is then nonlinearly scaled by the total contrast of the stimulus. Despite different roles of disparity (horizontal and vertical) and motion signals in visual scene analyses, the earliest processing stages of these different signals appear to be very similar.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近,在实验和概念上的努力已经复苏,以了解大脑节奏如何能够组织视觉信息。振荡可以为神经元处理提供时间结构,并形成整合大脑区域信息的基础。这里,我们使用双稳态范式和数据驱动方法来检验振荡调制与视觉元素的整合或分离相关的假设。在模棱两可和明确的条件下,使用脑磁图(MEG)研究了视觉运动刺激的绑定和未绑定配置感知的光谱特征。采用2×2设计,我们能够从视觉整合中分离出相关因素,感知或刺激驱动,与注意力和歧义相关的活动。发现通过视觉整合来调制两个频带:α/β频率和更高频率的γ频带。在视觉整合过程中,一些早期视觉皮层和背侧视觉区域的α/β功率增加,而在分离过程中,神经外视觉皮层的伽马带功率惊人地增加。这表明了α/β活性的综合作用,可能来自自上而下的信号,保持单一的视觉表示。另一方面,当更多的表示必须在并行伽马带活动中处理时,这与伽马振荡与感知相干性有关的概念不一致。这些调制在颅内脑电图记录中得到证实,部分起源于不同的大脑区域。我们的MEG和立体EEG数据证实了取决于低频活动的结合机制的预测,以进行远程整合和组织视觉处理,同时驳斥了伽马活动与感知结合之间的直接相关性。实践观点:不同的神经生理信号是竞争双稳态感知的基础。增加的α/β活性与视觉整合相关,而γ与分割相关。模糊的感知驱动后扣带皮质中的α/β活性。
    Recently, there has been a resurgence in experimental and conceptual efforts to understand how brain rhythms can serve to organize visual information. Oscillations can provide temporal structure for neuronal processing and form a basis for integrating information across brain areas. Here, we use a bistable paradigm and a data-driven approach to test the hypothesis that oscillatory modulations associate with the integration or segregation of visual elements. Spectral signatures of perception of bound and unbound configurations of visual moving stimuli were studied using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in ambiguous and unambiguous conditions. Using a 2 × 2 design, we were able to isolate correlates from visual integration, either perceptual or stimulus-driven, from attentional and ambiguity-related activity. Two frequency bands were found to be modulated by visual integration: an alpha/beta frequency and a higher frequency gamma-band. Alpha/beta power was increased in several early visual cortical and dorsal visual areas during visual integration, while gamma-band power was surprisingly increased in the extrastriate visual cortex during segregation. This points to an integrative role for alpha/beta activity, likely from top-down signals maintaining a single visual representation. On the other hand, when more representations have to be processed in parallel gamma-band activity is increased, which is at odds with the notion that gamma oscillations are related to perceptual coherence. These modulations were confirmed in intracranial EEG recordings and partially originate from distinct brain areas. Our MEG and stereo-EEG data confirms predictions of binding mechanisms depending on low-frequency activity for long-range integration and for organizing visual processing while refuting a straightforward correlation between gamma-activity and perceptual binding. PRACTITIONER POINTS: Distinct neurophysiological signals underlie competing bistable percepts. Increased alpha/beta activity correlate with visual integration while gamma correlates with segmentation. Ambiguous percepts drive alpha/beta activity in the posterior cingulate cortex.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    注意力和跨模态相互作用通过在感觉处理的不同阶段的复杂相互作用而紧密地联系在一起。在运动知觉的背景下,先前的研究表明,注意力需求会改变时域中的视听互动。在本研究中,我们的目的是了解这些注意力调节的神经生理学相关性。我们利用了视听运动范式,该范式引起了听觉时间间隔对感知视觉速度的影响。时域中的视听交互通过跨不同听觉时间间隔的感知视觉速度的变化来量化。我们通过在静止物体上执行次要任务来操纵视野中的注意力需求(即,single-vs.双重任务条件)。当注意力要求很高时(即,双重任务条件),听觉时间间隔对感知视觉速度的影响显着降低,这表明视听互动的减少。此外,我们发现视觉刺激在不同任务条件下引起的早期和晚期神经活动存在显着差异(单dual),反映了视觉领域注意力需求的全面增加。与感知视觉速度的变化一致,神经信号中的视听相互作用在晚期正成分范围内下降。与以前使用不同范式的研究结果相比,我们的研究结果支持这样的观点,即跨模态交互的注意力调节不是单一的,并且依赖于任务特定的成分.在感觉相关性和注意力需求不断变化的日常生活情况下,它们对于运动处理和速度估计也具有重要意义。
    Attention and crossmodal interactions are closely linked through a complex interplay at different stages of sensory processing. Within the context of motion perception, previous research revealed that attentional demands alter audiovisual interactions in the temporal domain. In the present study, we aimed to understand the neurophysiological correlates of these attentional modulations. We utilized an audiovisual motion paradigm that elicits auditory time interval effects on perceived visual speed. The audiovisual interactions in the temporal domain were quantified by changes in perceived visual speed across different auditory time intervals. We manipulated attentional demands in the visual field by having a secondary task on a stationary object (i.e., single- vs. dual-task conditions). When the attentional demands were high (i.e., dual-task condition), there was a significant decrease in the effects of auditory time interval on perceived visual speed, suggesting a reduction in audiovisual interactions. Moreover, we found significant differences in both early and late neural activities elicited by visual stimuli across task conditions (single vs. dual), reflecting an overall increase in attentional demands in the visual field. Consistent with the changes in perceived visual speed, the audiovisual interactions in neural signals declined in the late positive component range. Compared with the findings from previous studies using different paradigms, our findings support the view that attentional modulations of crossmodal interactions are not unitary and depend on task-specific components. They also have important implications for motion processing and speed estimation in daily life situations where sensory relevance and attentional demands constantly change.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近的研究表明,我们的大脑可能会产生微妙的,虚假的运动信号从其他人流向他们关注的对象,辅助社会认知。例如,短暂暴露于描绘其他人凝视着物体的静态图像,使得受试者在检测到随后的注视方向上的运动时变慢,这表明看着别人的凝视会导致定向运动适应。在这里我们确认,使用更严格的方法,观察另一个人注视特定方向的静态图像,在一个物体上,产生了相反方向的运动后遗症。后效表现为检测左与左的感知决策阈值的变化。右运动。当这个人远离物体时,效果消失了。这些发现表明,他人的专心注视被编码为隐含的代理到对象的运动,该运动足够健壮以引起真正的运动后效,虽然微妙到足以保持亚阈值。
    Recent work suggests that our brains may generate subtle, false motion signals streaming from other people to the objects of their attention, aiding social cognition. For instance, brief exposure to static images depicting other people gazing at objects made subjects slower at detecting subsequent motion in the direction of gaze, suggesting that looking at someone else\'s gaze caused a directional motion adaptation. Here we confirm, using a more stringent method, that viewing static images of another person gazing in a particular direction, at an object, produced motion aftereffects in the opposite direction. The aftereffect was manifested as a change in perceptual decision threshold for detecting left vs. right motion. The effect disappeared when the person was looking away from the object. These findings suggest that the attentive gaze of others is encoded as an implied agent-to-object motion that is sufficiently robust to cause genuine motion aftereffects, though subtle enough to remain subthreshold.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:为了更好地个性化治疗并监测腰痛患者的康复情况,感觉运动功能的客观测试,如腰椎本体感觉,必须根据它们的信度和效度进行选择。这项研究的主要目的是测试三种腰椎本体感觉方法的同时有效性。
    方法:31名参与者进行了三项腰椎本体感觉测试(运动知觉阈值,主动和被动关节定位感觉),整个身体的流动性和平衡(时间上升和运行)和两个躯干特定的姿势控制(稳定性阈值和基于传感器的摇摆措施)测试。
    结果:只有运动知觉阈值本体感觉测试显示出一定的有效性,与躯干特定姿势控制测试相关[r范围(正值):0.37至0.60]。三种腰椎本体感觉测量值彼此不相关。稳定性度量的阈值与时间上升(r=0.37)和特定于躯干的(基于传感器的摇摆度量)姿势控制[r范围(正值):0.48至0.77]测试相关。
    结论:本研究产生了三个原始发现。只有运动知觉阈值本体感觉测试证明了其并发有效性。事实上,本研究中进行的三项腰椎本体感觉测试彼此不相关,从而评估不同的结构。最后,针对其他测试验证了稳定性方案的阈值.这些发现将有助于选择最合适的腰椎本体感觉措施,以研究运动疗法对背痛患者的影响。
    BACKGROUND: To better personalize treatment and monitor recovery of individuals with low back pain, objective tests of sensorimotor functions, such as lumbar proprioception, must be selected based on their reliability and validity. The primary objective of this study was to test the concurrent validity of three measures of lumbar proprioception.
    METHODS: Thirty-one participants performed three lumbar proprioception tests (motion perception threshold, active and passive joint positioning sense), a whole-body mobility and balance (time up-and-go) and two trunk-specific postural control (threshold of stability and sensor-based sway measures) tests.
    RESULTS: Only the motion perception threshold proprioception test showed some validity, correlating with the trunk-specific postural control tests [r range (positive values): 0.37 to 0.60]. The three lumbar proprioception measures were not correlated to each other. The threshold of stability measure was correlated with the time up-and-go (r = 0.37) and trunk-specific (sensor-based sway measures) postural control [r range (positive values): 0.48 to 0.77] tests.
    CONCLUSIONS: The present study generated three original findings. Only the motion perception threshold proprioception test demonstrated its concurrent validity. In fact, the three lumbar proprioception tests performed in the present study were not correlated to each other, thus assessing different constructs. Finally, the threshold of stability protocol was validated against other tests. These findings will help in selecting the most appropriate lumbar proprioception measures to study the effects of exercise treatments in patients with back pain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    为了与世界成功互动,我们经常要评估自己的表现。虽然眼球运动是我们最常见的动作之一,我们通常不知道它们。这里,我们调查了是否有任何证据表明元认知敏感性对眼球运动的准确性。参与者跟踪点云,因为它遵循不可预测的正弦轨迹,然后报告他们是否认为他们的表现比他们的平均跟踪表现更好或更差。我们的结果表明,在所有试验中以及重复尝试相同目标轨迹时,都可以识别出更好的跟踪行为。区分更好和更差的试验表现的敏感性在各个阶段是稳定的,但是审判中的判决更多地依赖于最后几秒钟的表现。在判断手部动作质量时,这种行为与以前的报告一致,尽管总体上对眼球运动的元认知敏感性显着降低。
    For successful interactions with the world, we often have to evaluate our own performance. Although eye movements are one of the most frequent actions we perform, we are typically unaware of them. Here, we investigated whether there is any evidence for metacognitive sensitivity for the accuracy of eye movements. Participants tracked a dot cloud as it followed an unpredictable sinusoidal trajectory and then reported if they thought their performance was better or worse than their average tracking performance. Our results show above-chance identification of better tracking behavior across all trials and also for repeated attempts of the same target trajectories. Sensitivity in discriminating performance between better and worse trials was stable across sessions, but judgements within a trial relied more on performance in the final seconds. This behavior matched previous reports when judging the quality of hand movements, although overall metacognitive sensitivity for eye movements was significantly lower.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    运动可以在运动上或运动附近呈现的短暂闪烁测试的明显位置产生大的变化。这些运动引起的位置偏移可以具有各种来源。它们可能是由于帧效应,其中移动模式为其中的事件位置提供了参考帧。背景的运动可以通过跟踪其明确轮廓的高级机制起作用,或者运动可以通过来自低级运动检测器的信号作用于位置。在这里,我们通过消除明确的轮廓和可跟踪特征来隔离低级运动的贡献。在这种情况下,运动仍然支持探针位置的稳健移位,其中移位在跟随探针的运动方向上。虽然健壮,在我们的第一个实验中,位移的幅度约为先前研究中具有明确框架的位移的20%,在第二个,大约45%的发现有明确的框架。显然,单独的低级运动可以产生位置偏移,尽管与高级机制可以贡献的幅度相比,幅度大大降低。
    Motion can produce large changes in the apparent locations of briefly flashed tests presented on or near the motion. These motion-induced position shifts may have a variety of sources. They may be due to a frame effect where the moving pattern provides a frame of reference for the locations of events within it. The motion of the background may act through high-level mechanisms that track its explicit contours or the motion may act on position through the signals from low-level motion detectors. Here we isolate the contribution of low-level motion by eliminating explicit contours and trackable features. In this case, motion still supports a robust shift in probe locations with the shift being in the direction of the motion that follows the probe. Although robust, the magnitude of the shift in our first experiment is about 20% of the shift seen in a previous study with explicit frames and, in the second, about 45% of that found with explicit frames. Clearly, low-level motion alone can produce position shifts although the magnitude is much reduced compared to that seen when high-level mechanisms can contribute.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号