Moral emotions

道德情感
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前的研究表明,道德情绪在解释违反规则的行为时与自我控制和非结构化社交相互作用。高水平的道德情绪似乎会削弱自我控制和非结构化社交的影响,解释违反规则的行为。当前的研究检查了这些互动是否也影响与朋友明确承诺的违反规则的行为。此外,区分了道德情感的三种操作。使用自我报告问卷从N=169名青少年(54%为女性;平均=14.95岁;SD=1.7)收集数据。结果表明,道德冲突(AEMC)中高水平的预期情绪减弱了低自我控制对自己违反规则行为的影响。相比之下,高水平的内疚和羞愧倾向增强了非结构化社交对自己和与朋友打破规则的影响。研究的局限性,未来研究的想法,并讨论了实际意义。
    Previous research has shown that moral emotions interact with self-control and unstructured socializing in explaining rule-breaking behavior. High levels of moral emotions appear to weaken the effects of both self-control and unstructured socializing, in explaining rule-breaking behavior. The current study examined whether these interactions also affect rule-breaking behavior that is explicitly committed with friends. In addition, three operationalizations of moral emotions were distinguished. Data were collected from N = 169 adolescents (54% female; mean = 14.95 years; SD = 1.7) using a self-report questionnaire battery. Results indicate that high levels of anticipated emotions in moral conflicts (AEMC) attenuate the effect of low self-control on one\'s own rule-breaking behavior. In contrast, high levels of both guilt- and shame-proneness enhanced the effect of unstructured socializing on one\'s own and rule-breaking with friends. The limitations of the study, ideas for future research, and practical implications are also discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    道德伤害已成为理解道德违反后所经历的痛苦的重要建构,最初是在战斗退伍军人中,越来越多的是在其他人群中,例如医护人员和急救人员。虽然已经验证了许多措施来评估是否暴露于潜在的道德伤害事件和/或后遗症,需要额外的工具来促进对治疗性遭遇中道德伤害经验的细微差别讨论。道德伤害体验之轮(MIEW;弗莱明,2023)是一种信息图表工具,旨在引出精确的语言并帮助区分情感,以处理道德上令人困惑的情况。本文介绍了新开发的手册的内容和潜在的临床应用,以指导MIEW的使用。MIEW和手册旨在独立使用或与现有的道德伤害干预措施一起使用。使用MIEW和手册的案例研究演示了如何在专业医疗保健环境中使用这些工具。提供了对精神伤害护理从业人员的建议。
    Moral injury has emerged as an important construct for understanding the distress experienced in the aftermath of a moral violation, initially among combat veterans and increasingly among other populations, such as healthcare workers and first responders. While numerous measures have been validated to assess for exposure to potentially morally injurious events and/or sequelae, additional tools are needed to facilitate nuanced discussion of the experience of moral injury in therapeutic encounters. The Moral Injury Experience Wheel (MIEW; Fleming, 2023) is an infographic instrument that is designed to elicit precise language and help differentiate feelings in an effort to process morally perplexing circumstances. This paper describes the contents and potential clinical applications of a newly developed manual to guide the use of the MIEW. The MIEW and manual are designed to be used independently or alongside existing moral injury interventions. A case study featuring the use of the MIEW and manual demonstrates how the tools can be used in a professional healthcare setting. Recommendations for moral injury care practitioners are provided.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:道德困扰在新生儿重症监护病房(NICU)护士中很常见。目的:本研究的目的是评估NICU护士道德基础之间的关系,道德情感,和道德上的痛苦。研究设计和方法:这是一项观察性横断面自我报告问卷研究。参与者和研究背景:585名3-4级NICU护士中的一百四十二(24%)完成了道德基础的纸笔自我报告措施(危害,公平,分组,权威,和纯洁)(道德基础问卷-20),倾向于自我意识的道德情绪(内疚和羞耻)(修改后的个人感觉问卷-2),和道德困扰(徒劳的照顾,妥协的护理,和不真实的照顾)(修订后的道德困扰量表)。伦理考虑:参与是自愿和匿名的。参与医院的伦理委员会批准了研究方案(HREC参考:LNR/18/SCHN/316)。结果:非参数统计分析显示,道德基础与道德情感之间存在中等至大的相关性。道德基础和道德情感与道德困扰之间的联系微不足道。由于样本量小,使用<.10的自由p值进行统计显著性,伤害(rs=0.22)和公平(rs=0.16)预测了徒劳的护理,ingroup预测受损护理(rs=0.19)和不真实护理(rs=0.15),和纯度预测不真实的护理(rs=0.15)。内疚倾向预测徒劳的护理(rs=0.15)。羞耻感并不能预测道德上的痛苦。结论:道德基础与道德情绪存在显著相关性。道德基础和内疚倾向预测了道德困境的一个或多个方面。比预期小的效应大小可能是由于道德基础,道德情感,道德困境被概念化和衡量,或者道德上的脱离接触,包括新生儿重症监护病房的护士,可能不愿容忍厌恶,但在道德上有必要的负罪感,尤其是羞耻。了解这些关系的性质可能会补充NICU管理人员的努力,教育工作者,辅导员,和护士自己减轻道德困扰。
    Background: Moral distress is common in neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) nurses. Purpose: The purpose of this study was to evaluate the relationships between NICU nurses\' moral foundations, moral emotions, and moral distress. Research design and method: This is an observational cross-sectional self-report questionnaire study. Participants and research context: One hundred and forty-two (24%) of 585 Level 3-4 NICU nurses completed pen-and-paper self-report measures of moral foundations (harm, fairness, ingroup, authority, and purity) (Moral Foundations Questionnaire-20), proneness to self-conscious moral emotions (guilt and shame) (modified Personal Feelings Questionnaire-2), and moral distress (futile care, compromised care, and untruthful care) (modified Revised Moral Distress Scale). Ethical considerations: Participation was voluntary and anonymous. The ethics committees of the participating hospitals approved the study protocol (HREC Reference: LNR/18/SCHN/316). Results: Non-parametric statistical analyses showed medium to large correlations between moral foundations and moral emotions. Moral foundations and moral emotions had trivial to small correlations with moral distress. Using a liberal p-value of <.10 for statistical significance because of the small sample size, harm (rs = 0.22) and fairness (rs = 0.16) predicted futile care, ingroup predicted compromised care (rs = 0.19) and untruthful care (rs = 0.15), and purity predicted untruthful care (rs = 0.15). Guilt-proneness predicted futile care (rs = 0.15). Shame-proneness did not predict moral distress. Conclusion: The correlations between moral foundations and moral emotions were significant. Moral foundations and guilt-proneness predicted one or more dimensions of moral distress. The smaller than expected effect sizes may have been owing to how moral foundations, moral emotions, and moral distress were conceptualized and measured, or to moral disengagement, including NICU nurses\' possible reluctance to countenance aversive but morally warranted feelings of guilt and especially shame. Understanding the nature of these relationships may complement the efforts of NICU administrators, educators, counsellors, and nurses themselves to mitigate moral distress.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管具有无可争议的重要性,关于什么是情绪没有共识,大多数研究都是由特定的世界观来塑造他们的想法。这篇概念性文章探讨了对情感进行伊斯兰分析的案例。这似乎是一个需要检查的特殊领域;通过特定宗教的视角来分析这种普遍的心理概念有什么需要?三点用来论证这一努力。首先,这篇文章强调了这个词的相对不稳定性;关于什么是情绪,还没有普遍同意的定义,以及它们有助于哪些人类进程。由于概念仍在定义中,参与讨论是有好处的,特别是考虑到相对缺乏形而上学的辩论。其次,与情绪有关的特殊性以及它们是如何概念化的。本节深入研究可变因素的方式,比如文化,语言,和世界观,影响情绪如何表现。这里争论的最重要的一点是,情感是如何被体验的,即使经历了哪些情绪,是由在时间和空间上不一致的因素塑造的。因此,不同的世界观将形成不同的“情感味觉”和“情感意识形态”;不同的人群将不同地理解和参与情感。第三,随之而来的是关于科学本质的更广泛的讨论,心理学,以及它们与世俗性的关系。这种讨论包括对世俗主义和科学唯物主义等力量已被“发现”并因此是价值中立的观点的批评。因此,对学院出现的情感的理解,和当代心理学,充满价值。本节还着眼于科学的起源,以确定它本质上是无神论的还是宗教的,因此与宗教世界观对立。本节还挑战了心理学和关键心理学家的明显世俗性。一系列与情绪如何概念化有关的其他方面,比如他们的关系核心,他们与道德能力的互动,以及他们对个人和团体重要的东西的了解,也考虑到了。
    Despite being of undisputed importance, there is no consensus on what emotions are, with the majority of research that constructs ideas about them being colored by a particular worldview. This conceptual article examines the case for conducting an Islamic analysis of emotion. It might seem a peculiar area to examine; what would be the need to analyze such a universal psychological concept through the lens of a particular religion? Three points are used to argue for this endeavor. To begin with, this article highlights the relative instability of the term; there is yet no universally agreed upon definition of what emotions are, and which human processes they contribute to. As the concept is still being defined, there is merit in engaging with the discussion, particularly given the relative absence of metaphysics from the debate. Secondly, specificities relating to emotions and how they have conceptualized are considered. This section delves into the way in which variable factors, such as culture, language, and worldview, influence how emotions manifest. The overarching point argued for here is that how emotions are experienced, and even which emotions are experienced, are shaped by factors that are not consistent across time and space. Accordingly, different worldviews will formulate different \"emotional palates\" and \"emotional ideologies\"; different groups of people will understand and engage with emotions differently. Thirdly, a broader discussion ensues pertaining to the nature of science, psychology, and their relationship with secularity. This discussion includes critique of the idea that forces such as secularism and scientific materialism have been \"discovered\" and are therefore value-neutral. Accordingly, understandings of emotions to have emerged from the Academy, and contemporary psychology, are value-laden. This section also looks at the origins of science in order to determine whether it is inherently atheistic or areligious, and therefore antagonistic to a religious worldview. The section also challenges the apparent secularity of psychology and key psychologists. A range of other facets pertaining to how the emotions have been conceptualized, such as their relational core, their interaction with moral faculties, and their enmeshment with what is important to individuals and groups, are also considered.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    慈善事业对教育和研究等公共物品至关重要,艺术与文化,并为有需要的人提供服务。公共物品的提供者通常会在是否接受道德上受到污染的捐助者的捐赠的困境中挣扎。伦理学家也不同意如何管理受污染的捐赠。放弃这种捐赠会减少社会福祉和进步的机会;然而,接受它们会损害机构和个人的声誉。一半的专业筹款人面临着受污染的捐赠者,但只有大约三分之一的机构有相关政策(n=52)。这里,我们借鉴了两个大样本的美国外行人(ns=2,019;2,566)和一个独特的专家样本(专业筹款人,n=694),以提供对影响道德可接受性的受污染捐赠的各个方面的经验见解:道德污染的性质(犯罪或道德上模棱两可的行为),捐赠规模,匿名,和机构类型。我们发现了有趣的趋同模式(拒绝犯罪捐赠),分歧(专业人士对大量受污染捐赠的厌恶),以及样本中的冷漠(匿名的边缘角色)。外行人对大学和博物馆的标准也比对慈善机构的标准高。我们的结果提供了复杂的道德权衡如何不同地解决的证据,因此,可以激励和指导与有争议的捐助者打交道的机构制定政策。
    Philanthropy is essential to public goods such as education and research, arts and culture, and the provision of services to those in need. Providers of public goods commonly struggle with the dilemma of whether to accept donations from morally tainted donors. Ethicists also disagree on how to manage tainted donations. Forgoing such donations reduces opportunities for societal well-being and advancement; however, accepting them can damage institutional and individual reputations. Half of professional fundraisers have faced tainted donors, but only around a third of their institutions had relevant policies (n = 52). Here, we draw on two large samples of US laypeople (ns = 2,019; 2,566) and a unique sample of experts (professional fundraisers, n = 694) to provide empirical insights into various aspects of tainted donations that affect moral acceptability: the nature of the moral taint (criminal or morally ambiguous behavior), donation size, anonymity, and institution type. We find interesting patterns of convergence (rejecting criminal donations), divergence (professionals\' aversion to large tainted donations), and indifference (marginal role of anonymity) across the samples. Laypeople also applied slightly higher standards to universities and museums than to charities. Our results provide evidence of how complex moral trade-offs are resolved differentially, and can thus motivate and inform policy development for institutions dealing with controversial donors.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    社区特征可以解释学校之间欺凌率的差异。在社会生态模式的基础上,这项研究调查了社区暴力暴露(CVE)之间的关系,道德情感(内疚和同情),和欺凌青少年。我们还探讨了性别在这些关系中的调节作用。样本包括915名11-16岁的青少年(48.6%的女性和51.4%的男性)(M年龄=13.76,SD=0.82)。学生对自我报告措施做出了回应。我们使用潜在变量结构方程模型(SEM)方法来检查变量之间的关系。SEM表明,CVE与道德情绪(内和同情)呈负相关,与欺凌呈正相关。此外,CVE通过与道德情绪的负面关系与欺凌有负面间接关联。结构模型对两种性别都是等效的,表明性别并不能缓和这些关系。这些发现表明,CVE与学校欺凌率的差异有关。
    Community characteristics can explain differences in bullying rates between schools. Underpinned by the social-ecological model, this study examined the relationships among community violence exposure (CVE), moral emotions (guilt and sympathy), and bullying in adolescents. We also explored the moderating role of gender in these relationships. The sample included 915 adolescents (48.6% female and 51.4% male) aged 11-16 (M age = 13.76, SD = 0.82). Students responded to the self-report measures. We used a latent variable structural equation modeling (SEM) approach to examine the relationships between variables. The SEM indicated that CVE was negatively associated with moral emotions (guilt and sympathy) and positively associated with bullying. In addition, CVE had a negative indirect association with bullying through their negative relationship with moral emotions. The structural model is equivalent for both genders, suggesting gender does not moderate these relationships. These findings indicate that CVE are associated with differences in school bullying rates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前对道德判断(MJ)的研究集中在理解影响不同类型道德判断任务的认知过程和情感因素上,例如个人和非个人的困境。然而,很少有研究区分与认知相关的情绪和由MJ任务引起的复杂情绪。知识上的这种差距对于解决更好地了解情绪如何影响道德判断很重要。这项研究的目的是调查恐惧的影响以及道德情绪对MJ的作用。通过jsPsych从145名参与者中收集数据,并使用混合模型方差分析(ANOVA)和相关分析进行分析。研究发现,恐惧引发的个体增加了个人道德情景中功利主义道德判断的数量,延长了认知过程,但不是在非个人的道德困境中。因此,我们推测,恐惧可能在个人道德困境中起着认知作用,而在非个人道德困境中起着情感作用。另一个发现是,道德决策过程中产生的复杂道德情绪可能会影响恐惧的有效性,并可能影响道德判断。然而,这项研究对这些发现持谨慎态度,并在未来进一步验证这一假设。
    Previous research on moral judgment (MJ) has focused on understanding the cognitive processes and emotional factors that influence different types of moral judgment tasks, such as personal and impersonal dilemmas. However, few studies have distinguished between the emotions related to cognition and the complex emotions specifically caused by MJ tasks. This gap in knowledge is important to address to have a better understanding of how emotions influence moral judgment. The aim of this study was to investigate the influence of fear and the role of moral emotions on MJ. Data were collected from 145 participants through jsPsych and analyzed using mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) and correlation analysis. The study found that individuals who were triggered by the fear increased the number of utilitarian moral judgments in personal moral scenarios and lengthened the cognitive process, but not in impersonal moral dilemmas. Hence, we speculate that fear may play a cognitive role in personal moral dilemmas and an emotional role in impersonal moral dilemmas. Another finding is that the complex moral emotions arising from the moral decision-making process may affect the effectiveness of fear and potentially influence moral judgments. However, this study adopts a cautious attitude toward these discoveries, and further verification of this hypothesis should be conducted in the future.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    道德和自我意识的情绪,如内疚和羞耻,可以作为内部的负面经验,惩罚或阻止不良行为。个体差异存在于人们体验内疚和羞愧的倾向上。倾向于经历内疚和/或羞耻可以预测学生对他们的情绪反应的期望,以学术不端行为的形式从事不道德行为,从而劝阻学生不要有意从事这种行为。在这项研究中,学生(n=459)内疚和羞愧倾向,如果他们从事学术不端行为,他们会感到内疚和羞耻,并测量了他们从事学术不端行为的意图。内疚和羞耻倾向量表的四个方面中的三个[GASP:内疚-消极-行为-评估(NBE),内疚修复,羞耻-消极-自我评价(NSE)]与学术不端行为意图呈显著负相关,这些关系是通过预期与从事学术不端行为有关的羞耻和内疚来调解的。这些结果表明,对于一些希望在从事学术不端行为时经历负面道德情绪的学生来说,可能会保护他们免受违反道德评估规则的影响。
    Moral and self-conscious emotions like guilt and shame can function as internal negative experiences that punish or deter bad behaviour. Individual differences exist in people\'s tendency to experience guilt and shame. Being disposed to experience guilt and/or shame may predict students\' expectations of their emotional reactions to engaging in immoral behaviour in the form of academic misconduct, and thus dissuade students from intending to engage in this behaviour. In this study, students\' (n = 459) guilt and shame proneness, their expectations of feeling guilt and shame if they engaged in academic misconduct, and their intentions to engage in academic misconduct were measured. Three of the four facets of the guilt and shame proneness scale [GASP: Guilt-Negative-Behavior-Evaluation (NBE), Guilt-Repair, Shame-Negative-Self-Evaluation (NSE)] had significant negative correlations with academic misconduct intentions, and these relationships were mediated by anticipating shame and guilt related to engaging in academic misconduct. These results suggest that for some students expecting to experience negative moral emotions when engaging in academic misconduct may protect them from breaching ethical assessment rules.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    道德信仰激励行动吗?要回答这个问题,现存的论点考虑了代理人道德信念和行为之间的关联(分离)的假设案例。在本文中,我认为,可以通过使用实证研究方法研究人们的实际道德信念和行为来改进这种方法。我提出了三项新的研究表明,当赌注很高时,参与者的道德信念和行为之间的联系实际上是由共同发生但独立的道德情感来解释的。这些发现表明,道德信仰本身几乎没有动机,支持道德动机的胡意思图。
    Do moral beliefs motivate action? To answer this question, extant arguments have considered hypothetical cases of association (dissociation) between agents\' moral beliefs and actions. In this paper, I argue that this approach can be improved by studying people\'s actual moral beliefs and actions using empirical research methods. I present three new studies showing that, when the stakes are high, associations between participants\' moral beliefs and actions are actually explained by co-occurring but independent moral emotions. These findings suggest that moral beliefs themselves have little or no motivational force, supporting the Humean picture of moral motivation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自我意识的情绪,比如羞耻和内疚,在规范道德行为和促进社会福利方面发挥根本作用。尽管它们具有相关性,这些情绪的神经基础是不确定的。在目前的荟萃分析中,我们进行了系统的文献综述,以找出对健康个体的功能神经影像学研究,特别是研究羞耻的神经底物,尴尬,和内疚。17项研究研究了羞耻/尴尬的神经相关性,17项研究了内感大脑表现,符合我们的纳入标准。分析显示,内疚和羞耻/尴尬都与左前脑岛的激活有关,参与情绪意识处理和唤醒。内疚的特定区域位于左颞顶叶交界处,被认为参与了社会认知过程。此外,羞耻/尴尬的特定激活涉及与社交疼痛(背侧前扣带和丘脑)和行为抑制(运动前皮层)网络相关的区域。这种结果模式可能反映了与这两种情绪相关的不同的动作趋势。
    Self-conscious emotions, such as shame and guilt, play a fundamental role in regulating moral behaviour and in promoting the welfare of society. Despite their relevance, the neural bases of these emotions are uncertain. In the present meta-analysis, we performed a systematic literature review in order to single out functional neuroimaging studies on healthy individuals specifically investigating the neural substrates of shame, embarrassment, and guilt. Seventeen studies investigating the neural correlates of shame/embarrassment and seventeen studies investigating guilt brain representation met our inclusion criteria. The analyses revealed that both guilt and shame/embarrassment were associated with the activation of the left anterior insula, involved in emotional awareness processing and arousal. Guilt-specific areas were located within the left temporo-parietal junction, which is thought to be involved in social cognitive processes. Moreover, specific activations for shame/embarrassment involved areas related to social pain (dorsal anterior cingulate and thalamus) and behavioural inhibition (premotor cortex) networks. This pattern of results might reflect the distinct action tendencies associated with the two emotions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号