Masking

遮蔽
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:自闭症特征的伪装涉及隐藏或补偿自闭症特征,通常是由于耻辱或渴望与他人相处。这种行为与自闭症患者的心理健康问题有关。2.5项伪装自闭症特征问卷(CAT-Q)是最常用的伪装自我报告措施。在这项研究中,我们开发了9个项目的简短版本,用于临床和研究环境.
    目的:构建并在心理上验证一个简短的自我报告伪装措施。
    方法:开发了伪装孤独症特质问卷(CATQ-SF),并在两项研究中评估了其因子结构和心理测量特性。研究1使用了一个大的,自闭症和非自闭症成年人的在线样本(N=832)来评估因子结构,心理测量属性,和CATQ-SF的测量不变性。研究2使用自闭症和非自闭症成年人的独立样本(N=80)来测试研究1的发现。
    结果:在研究1中,观察到三因素结构的证据,具有良好的内部一致性(自闭症和非自闭症组合α=0.84)。此外,仪器证明了测量不变性,并可靠地预测了更高水平的自闭症特征。在研究2中,三因素结构被复制,再次观察到良好的内部一致性(自闭症和非自闭症α=0.89)。在两项研究中,与完整形式的CAT-Q相比,心理测量特性具有相似或更高的有效性。
    结论:临床医生和研究人员可以使用CATQ-SF快速可靠地测量自闭症和非自闭症成年人的伪装情况。
    BACKGROUND: Camouflaging of autistic traits involves hiding or compensating for autistic characteristics, often due to stigma or a desire to fit in with others. This behaviour has been associated with mental health issues in autistic individuals. The 2 5-item Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q) is the most commonly used self-report measure of camouflaging. In this study, a 9-item short form version was developed for use in clinical and research settings.
    OBJECTIVE: To construct and psychometrically validate a brief self-report measure of camouflaging.
    METHODS: The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire - Short Form (CATQ-SF) was developed and its factor structure and psychometric properties were evaluated in two studies. Study 1 used a large, online sample of autistic and non-autistic adults (N = 832) to evaluate the factor structure, psychometric properties, and measurement invariance of the CATQ-SF. Study 2 used an independent sample of autistic and non-autistic adults (N = 80) to test Study 1\'s findings.
    RESULTS: In Study 1, evidence for a three-factor structure was observed, with good internal consistency (combined autistic & non-autistic α = 0.84). In addition, the instrument demonstrated measurement invariance, and reliably predicted higher levels of autistic traits. In Study 2, the 3-factor structure was replicated, and good internal consistency was again observed (combined autistic and non-autistic α = 0.89). In both studies, psychometric properties were of similar or higher validity compared to the full-form CAT-Q.
    CONCLUSIONS: The CATQ-SF can be used by clinicians and researchers to measure camouflaging in autistic and non-autistic adults quickly and reliably.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:这项研究调查了平行刺激呈现对人类听众听觉脑干反应(ABR)的位置特异性的影响。特定频率的刺激并不能保证仅对应于该特征频率的耳蜗位置的反应-特别是对于短暂和高水平的刺激。添加掩蔽噪声会产生更特定于地点的响应,并且我们先前的建模研究表明,当并行呈现多个频率特定刺激时,效果相似。我们在这里实验测试了这个假设,比较在两个刺激频率和三个刺激速率下对串行和并行刺激的反应的位置特异性。
    方法:平行ABR(pABR)刺激与高通滤波噪声一起呈现,并具有不同的截止频率。还通过从pABR集合中分离和呈现单频刺激序列来测试串行呈现。检查ABR的潜伏期以评估反应的位置特异性。通过减去来自不同高通噪声条件的响应来得出响应带。然后使用来自每个导出的响应带的响应幅度来确定听觉系统的各个频率区域对总体响应有多少贡献。
    结果:我们发现,对于较低的刺激频率和较高的刺激速率,平行呈现提高了ABR的位置特异性。在更高的刺激频率下,串行和并行演示同样具体的地方。
    结论:对于较低的刺激频率,并行呈现可以提供比串行更多的特定于地点的响应。这种改善随着更高的刺激速率而增加,并且是pABR的主要好处,即更快的测试时间。
    OBJECTIVE: This study investigates the effect of parallel stimulus presentation on the place specificity of the auditory brainstem response (ABR) in human listeners. Frequency-specific stimuli do not guarantee a response from the place on the cochlea corresponding only to that characteristic frequency - especially for brief and high-level stimuli. Adding masking noise yields responses that are more place specific, and our prior modeling study has suggested similar effects when multiple frequency-specific stimuli are presented in parallel. We tested this hypothesis experimentally here, comparing the place specificity of responses to serial and parallel stimuli at two stimulus frequencies and three stimulus rates.
    METHODS: Parallel ABR (pABR) stimuli were presented alongside high-pass filtered noise with a varied cutoff frequency. Serial presentation was also tested by isolating and presenting single-frequency stimulus trains from the pABR ensemble. Latencies of the ABRs were examined to assess place specificity of responses. Response bands were derived by subtracting responses from different high-pass noise conditions. The response amplitude from each derived response band was then used to determine how much individual frequency regions of the auditory system were contributing to the overall response.
    RESULTS: We found that parallel presentation improves place specificity of ABRs for the lower stimulus frequency and at higher stimulus rates. At a higher stimulus frequency, serial and parallel presentations were equally place specific.
    CONCLUSIONS: Parallel presentation can provide more place-specific responses than serial for lower stimulus frequencies. The improvement increases with higher stimulus rates and is in addition to the pABR\'s primary benefit of faster test times.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自闭症掩蔽是指一些自闭症个体隐藏的倾向,压制,或者伪装他们的自闭症特征,自闭症身份,或自闭症诊断。自闭症掩蔽也可能包括无意识或有意识的模仿行为的尝试,认知,或非自闭症神经典型人群的感觉风格,并抑制自闭症行为的自然形式,认知,以及对感官体验的反应。由于自闭症患者在许多神经典型的社会中都是被污名化的少数群体,作为非自闭症患者通过自闭症掩盖可能是一种尝试,以避免自闭症的污名化和对以前的人际创伤的反应。自闭症掩蔽行为的增加与抑郁症增加的报告有关,焦虑,倦怠,自闭症患者的疲惫,因此,探索自闭症掩蔽的根源和影响是一个重要的心理健康课题。
    这项研究调查了自闭症掩蔽和抑郁症之间的关系,焦虑,性别认同,性取向,人际关系创伤,自尊,真实性,和自闭症社区的参与。参与者是通过自闭症社交媒体团体招募的自闭症成年人(n=342)。
    这项研究发现,自我报告较高的自闭症掩蔽行为与过去人际关系创伤的较高报告相关,更严重的焦虑和抑郁症状,较低的自尊,较低的真实性,自闭症社区的参与度较低。自闭症掩蔽与性别认同或性取向无关。
    这项研究的结果突出了自闭症掩盖与过去的人际创伤之间的关系,发现自闭症掩蔽行为与心理健康有关,自尊,和自闭症成年人的真实性风险。根据本研究和该领域其他研究中提出的与自闭症掩蔽的负面关联,我们对自闭症患者的治疗和教育计划中的自闭症掩蔽策略的教学提出了质疑。
    为什么这是一个重要的问题?自闭症掩蔽是指自闭症患者抑制其自然自闭症特征,回应,和行为,在尝试中,有意识或无意识地,隐藏或减少他们自闭症特征的可见度,自闭症身份,或自闭症诊断。高水平的自闭症掩盖与负面的心理健康有关,真实性挑战,自闭症患者的倦怠。人际压力,过去的创伤社会经历,自闭症的污名可能会助长自闭症的掩盖。这项研究的目的是什么?这项研究旨在探讨自闭症掩蔽与抑郁症之间的关系。焦虑,人际关系创伤,自尊,真实性,自闭症社区的参与,性别认同,和性取向。研究人员做了什么?我们通过Facebook上的自闭症社交媒体团体招募了342名自闭症成年参与者,完成了一项30分钟的在线匿名调查,其中包括经过验证的量表来测量自闭症掩盖,抑郁症,焦虑,人际关系创伤,自尊,和真实性。关于人口因素的其他问题被问到,比如性别认同和性取向,我们问了一些关于过去社会创伤和交叉问题的开放式问题。研究结果是什么?我们发现,自我报告较高的自闭症掩蔽行为与过去人际关系创伤的较高报告相关,特别是被自闭症特征羞辱和嘲笑,以及更广泛的情感和身体虐待经历。蒙面还与更大的焦虑和更多的抑郁症状有关,较低的自尊,较低的真实生活,更多地接受外部影响,更高的自我疏离,自闭症社区的参与度较低。未发现自闭症掩盖与性别认同或性取向有关。报告参与先前应用行为分析治疗的参与者报告说,过去的人际关系创伤比参与认知行为治疗等其他形式治疗的参与者更高。这项研究支持了先前将自闭症掩盖与抑郁和焦虑症状联系起来的研究,和较低的报告真实性,比如自闭症患者觉得他们不忠于自己,或者向别人展示他们真正的自我。这项研究是第一个定量调查自闭症掩盖和过去的人际创伤之间的关系,自尊,真实性,和自闭症社区的参与。这项研究的潜在弱点是什么?我们的样本在种族方面并不代表美国人口,教育水平,性别,和性取向。它非常白,受过高等教育,很少有顺性人,百分之六十三是性少数群体的成员。大多数参与者报告了自闭症的晚期诊断。与一般自闭症人群相比,该样本可能包含大量自闭症掩蔽水平高的人,或者最近意识到自己是自闭症掩蔽的人。我们没有分析早期诊断和晚期诊断队列之间的差异。这些发现将如何帮助现在或将来的自闭症成年人?这项研究质疑自闭症掩盖策略在育儿中的教学,教育,以及基于自闭症掩蔽的负面关联的自闭症患者治疗计划。我们的发现应被用作强烈的行动呼吁,以抵制鼓励自闭症掩盖和自闭症特质羞辱的做法。相反,我们提倡提倡养育子女的形式,教育,和治疗尊重自闭症患者的特点,沟通风格,感官需要,和自闭症身份。
    UNASSIGNED: Autistic masking refers to some autistic individuals\' tendency to hide, suppress, or camouflage their autistic traits, autistic identity, or autism diagnosis. Autistic masking also may include unconscious or conscious attempts to mimic the behavioral, cognitive, or sensory styles of nonautistic neurotypical people and to suppress natural forms of autistic behavior, cognition, and reactions to sensory experiences. Since autistic people are a stigmatized minority in many neurotypical dominated societies, passing as nonautistic through autistic masking may be an attempt to avoid autism stigma and a reaction to previous interpersonal trauma. Increased autistic masking behaviors are associated with reports of increased depression, anxiety, burnout, and exhaustion in autistic people, and thus, exploring the roots and impact of autistic masking is an important mental health topic.
    UNASSIGNED: This study investigated the relationships between autistic masking and depression, anxiety, gender identity, sexual orientation, interpersonal trauma, self-esteem, authenticity, and autistic community involvement. Participants were autistic adults (n = 342) recruited through autistic social media groups.
    UNASSIGNED: This study found that higher self-reported autistic masking behaviors were associated with higher reports of past interpersonal trauma, greater anxiety and depression symptoms, lower self-esteem, lower authenticity, and lower participation within the autistic community. Autistic masking was not associated with gender identity or sexual orientation.
    UNASSIGNED: The results of this study highlight the relationship between autistic masking and past interpersonal trauma, finding that autistic masking behavior is associated with mental health, self-esteem, and authenticity risks for autistic adults. We call into question the teaching of autistic masking strategies in therapies and education programs for autistic people based on the negative associations with autistic masking presented in this study and other research in this field.
    Why is this an important issue? Autistic masking refers to autistic people suppressing their natural autistic traits, responses, and behaviors, in an attempt, consciously or unconsciously, to hide or reduce the visibility of their autistic traits, autistic identity, or autism diagnosis. High levels of autistic masking are associated with negative mental health, authenticity challenges, and burnout for autistic people. Interpersonal pressuring, past traumatic social experiences, and autism stigma potentially fuel autistic masking. What is the purpose of this study? This study aimed to investigate relationship between autistic masking and depression, anxiety, interpersonal trauma, self-esteem, authenticity, autistic community involvement, gender identity, and sexual orientation. What did the researchers do? We recruited 342 autistic adult participants through autistic social media groups on Facebook to complete a 30-minute anonymous survey online comprising validated scales to measure autistic masking, depression, anxiety, interpersonal trauma, self-esteem, and authenticity. Additional questions were asked about demographic factors, such as gender identity and sexual orientation, and we asked open-ended questions about past social trauma and intersectional issues. What were the results of the study? We found that higher self-reported autistic masking behaviors were associated with higher reports of past interpersonal trauma, specifically being shamed and teased about autistic traits, and broader experiences of emotional and physical abuse. Masking was also associated with greater anxiety and more depression symptoms, lower self-esteem, lower authentic living, greater accepting of external influence, higher self-alienation, and lower participation within the autistic community. Autistic masking was not found to be associated with gender identity or sexual orientation. Participants who reported involvement in previous applied behavior analysis therapy reported higher past interpersonal trauma than participants involved in some other forms of therapy such as cognitive behavior therapy. What do the findings add to what was already known? This study supports previous research associating autistic masking with depression and anxiety symptoms, and lower reported authenticity, such as autistic people feeling they were not being true to themselves, or revealing their genuine selves to others. This study is the first to quantitatively investigate relationships between autistic masking and past interpersonal traumas, self-esteem, authenticity, and autistic community involvement. What are potential weaknesses in the study? Our sample is not representative of the U.S. population when it comes to race, educational level, gender, and sexual orientation. It was very White, highly educated, had few cisgender men, and sixty three percent were members of sexual minority groups. The majority of participants reported late diagnosis of autism. This sample potentially contained an overrepresentation of people with high levels of autistic masking or who more recently realized they were autistic masking in comparison with the general autistic population. We did not analyze differences between early-diagnosed and late-diagnosed cohorts. How will these findings help autistic adults now or in the future? This research calls into question the teaching of autistic masking strategies in parenting, education, and therapy programs for autistic people based on the negative associations of autistic masking. Our findings should be utilized as a strong call to action to push back against practices that encourage autistic masking and autistic trait shaming. Instead we advocate for promoting forms of parenting, education, and therapy that respect autistic people\'s traits, communication styles, sensory needs, and autistic identity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:肾细胞癌(RCC)由于其存活率低,仍然是全球健康问题。本研究旨在探讨医学决定因素和社会经济状况对RCC患者生存结局的影响。我们分析了监测下记录的41,563例RCC患者的生存数据,流行病学,和2012年至2020年的最终结果(SEER)计划。
    方法:我们采用了竞争风险模型,假设不同风险下的RCC患者的生存期遵循Chen分布。该模型解释了与生存时间以及死亡原因相关的不确定性,包括失踪的死因.对于模型分析,我们利用贝叶斯推断,获得了累积发生率函数(CIF)和特定原因危险等各种关键参数的估计值.此外,我们采用贝叶斯假设检验来评估多因素对RCC患者生存时间的影响.
    结果:我们的研究结果表明,肾癌患者的生存时间受性别的显著影响,收入,婚姻状况,化疗,肿瘤大小,和偏侧性。然而,我们观察到种族和起源对患者的生存时间没有显著影响。CIF图表明,与收入因素相对应的死亡原因发生率存在许多重要差异,婚姻状况,种族,化疗,和肿瘤大小。
    结论:本研究强调了各种医学和社会经济因素对RCC患者生存时间的影响。此外,这也证明了在贝叶斯范式下竞争风险模型在RCC患者生存分析中的实用性。该模型提供了一个强大而灵活的框架来处理丢失的数据,这在患者信息可能不完整的现实生活中特别有用。
    BACKGROUND: Renal cell carcinoma (RCC) remains a global health concern due to its poor survival rate. This study aimed to investigate the influence of medical determinants and socioeconomic status on survival outcomes of RCC patients. We analyzed the survival data of 41,563 RCC patients recorded under the Surveillance, Epidemiology, and End Results (SEER) program from 2012 to 2020.
    METHODS: We employed a competing risk model, assuming lifetime of RCC patients under various risks follows Chen distribution. This model accounts for uncertainty related to survival time as well as causes of death, including missing cause of death. For model analysis, we utilized Bayesian inference and obtained the estimate of various key parameters such as cumulative incidence function (CIF) and cause-specific hazard. Additionally, we performed Bayesian hypothesis testing to assess the impact of multiple factors on the survival time of RCC patients.
    RESULTS: Our findings revealed that the survival time of RCC patients is significantly influenced by gender, income, marital status, chemotherapy, tumor size, and laterality. However, we observed no significant effect of race and origin on patient\'s survival time. The CIF plots indicated a number of important distinctions in incidence of causes of death corresponding to factors income, marital status, race, chemotherapy, and tumor size.
    CONCLUSIONS: The study highlights the impact of various medical and socioeconomic factors on survival time of RCC patients. Moreover, it also demonstrates the utility of competing risk model for survival analysis of RCC patients under Bayesian paradigm. This model provides a robust and flexible framework to deal with missing data, which can be particularly useful in real-life situations where patients information might be incomplete.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    许多自闭症患者使用被称为“伪装”的策略来改变他们的自闭症特征在社交场合中的明显程度。先前的研究表明,伪装在很大程度上是由心理和社会因素引起的。然而,到目前为止,大多数研究只研究了一些与伪装相关的社会心理因素。在这项研究中,我们探索了一个包含几个个体心理因素的模型(比如害怕被负面评价,自尊和自闭症身份)和更广泛的社会和文化因素(如感知的污名,负面生活事件,文化强调一致性和适应或脱颖而出的愿望)。我们在网上调查了225名18-77岁的自闭症成年人。我们的发现表明,一些社会文化因素通过个人心理因素与伪装间接相关。害怕被负面评价是伪装的强烈预测因素。具体来说,自闭症成年人感到更大的耻辱,感到更大的压力,有较小的脱颖而出的欲望和更大的适应欲望倾向于经历更大的恐惧被负面判断和报道更多的伪装。此外,那些经历了更多负面生活事件的人更有可能进行伪装。我们的研究将关键的心理和社会因素确定为社会变革的潜在目标。我们的研究结果强调,我们的社会需要从污名化的态度转向接受和包括自闭症患者,这可以减轻自闭症患者在社交场合伪装的压力。
    UNASSIGNED: Many autistic people use strategies known as \'camouflaging\' to change how noticeable their autistic traits are in social situations. Previous research suggests that camouflaging is largely motivated by psychological and social factors. However, most studies so far have only looked at a few psychosocial factors related to camouflaging. In this study, we explored a model that included several individual psychological factors (such as fear of being negatively judged, self-esteem and autistic identity) and broader social and cultural factors (such as perceived stigma, negative life events, cultural emphasis on conformity and desire to fit in or stand out). We surveyed 225 autistic adults aged 18-77 years online. Our findings showed that several sociocultural factors were indirectly linked to camouflaging through individual psychological factors. Fear of being negatively judged emerged as a strong predictor of camouflaging. Specifically, autistic adults who perceived greater stigma, felt greater pressure to conform, had a lesser desire to stand out and a greater desire to fit in tended to experience a greater fear of being negatively judged and reported more camouflaging. In addition, those who experienced more negative life events were more likely to engage in camouflaging. Our study identifies key psychological and social factors as potential targets for social change. Our findings emphasise that our societies need to shift away from stigmatising attitudes towards accepting and including autistic people, which could reduce the pressure on autistic individuals to camouflage in social situations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    据报道,许多自闭症患者从事伪装以浏览日常社交互动;然而,这种行为的功能在很大程度上仍然未知。我们假设自闭症患者比自闭症患者更倾向于神经典型的其他人,将其作为“适应”神经典型-占多数社区的一种策略。本研究旨在首次对这一假设进行实证研究。自闭症和神经典型的参与者参加了一项基于网络的调查。分析了48名自闭症患者和137名神经典型参与者的数据。使用改良的伪装自闭症特征问卷(CAT-Q)分别测量了对自闭症和神经典型其他人的伪装。对于每个CAT-Q项目,增加了描述具有自闭症或神经典型特征的假设互动伴侣的句子,创造各自的句子条件。参与者特征和句子条件的交互效应采用多水平回归分析,考虑不同的个别基线。分析揭示了参与者特征与句子条件之间的相互作用。自闭症组在自闭症句子状况下的伪装明显高于神经典型句子状况。相反,在句子条件下,神经典型组的伪装水平没有显著差异.与我们的假设相反,自闭症患者对自闭症患者的伪装程度高于对神经典型患者的伪装程度。这一发现质疑自闭症患者伪装成神经典型的大多数社会的假设。相反,它可以被概念化为自闭症患者旨在改善与他人关系的更一般的社会策略。
    Many autistic people reportedly engage in camouflaging to navigate everyday social interactions; however, the function of this behavior remains largely unknown. We hypothesized that autistic people camouflage more toward neurotypical others than toward autistic others, employing it as a strategy to \"fit in\" within the neurotypical-majority community. This study aimed to empirically investigate this hypothesis for the first time. Autistic and neurotypical participants took part in a web-based survey. Data from 48 autistic and 137 neurotypical participants were analyzed. Camouflaging toward autistic and neurotypical others was separately measured using the modified Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q). For each CAT-Q item, a sentence describing a hypothetical interaction partner with autistic or neurotypical characteristics was added, creating respective sentence conditions. The interaction effect of the participants\' characteristics and sentence conditions was analyzed using a multilevel regression analysis, accounting for differing individual baselines. The analysis revealed an interaction effect between participants\' characteristics and sentence conditions. The autistic group showed significantly more camouflaging in the autistic sentence condition than in the neurotypical sentence condition. Conversely, the neurotypical group did not differ significantly in camouflaging levels in the sentence conditions. Contrary to our hypothesis, autistic people demonstrated more camouflaging toward autistic others than toward neurotypical others. This finding questions the assumption that autistic people camouflage to assimilate into a neurotypical-majority society. Instead, it could be conceptualized as a more general social strategy used by autistic people aiming to improve their relationships with others.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Editorial
    随机临床试验提供了这样的保证,即混杂因素在基线时是平衡的,而盲法对于确保此后无关因素的平衡至关重要。本文提供了由三部分组成的分类方法,对由于临床试验中盲法不足而可能出现的陷阱进行分类。我们为读者介绍了一个警示框架,以解释循证医学的盲法随机试验。每个陷阱都用一个相关的示例来说明,该示例是由于了解组分配而产生的潜在偏差。在研究过程中出现了一些陷阱,包括干预组的盲法不足,对照组,或负责任的临床医生。其他陷阱与数据分析有关,包括对盲法的未经证实的断言和对盲法的颠覆测试。由于周围的监督,包括研究伦理委员会和科学审查员的揭盲,出现了进一步的陷阱。当观察临床干预与患者结果之间的明显联系时,这些警告是误解的根源。对特定陷阱的认识可能有助于推进盲法随机临床试验的解释和应用,以告知循证医疗服务。
    Randomized clinical trials provide reassurances that confounding factors are balanced at baseline whereas blinding is essential to assure the balance of extraneous factors thereafter. This article provides a three-part taxonomy of pitfalls that can arise because of inadequate blinding in clinical trials. We introduce a cautionary framework for readers interpreting a blinded randomized trial for evidence-based medicine. Each pitfall is illustrated with a relevant example of a potential bias resulting from knowledge of group assignment. Several pitfalls occur during the conduct of the study including inadequate blinding of the intervention group, control group, or responsible clinicians. Additional pitfalls relate to data analysis including unsubstantiated assertions of blinding and subverted tests for blinding. Further pitfalls arise due to surrounding oversight including unblinding of research ethics boards and scientific reviewers. These caveats are sources of misunderstanding when observing the apparent connection between a clinical intervention and patient outcomes. An awareness of specific pitfalls might help advance the interpretation and application of blinded randomized clinical trials to inform evidence-based medical care.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    酵母提取物(YEs)由于其风味特性和减少苦味的能力而用于食品中。已知在YEs中发现的腺苷5'-单磷酸(AMP)可降低某些化合物的苦味。本研究旨在使用体外基于细胞的测定来研究YEs抑制苦味受体(TAS2R)的能力。由AMP和YEs激活的TAS2R的筛选显示AMP和富含AMP的YE激活了更多的TAS2R。研究了富含AMP的YE对苦味激动剂激活的7种TAS2R的抑制作用。YE降低了TAS2R的激活,增加了EC50值,减小了最大振幅,表现出竞争性和非竞争性抑制。在测试的19个TAS2R中,七个在处理富含AMP的YE后显示出40%或更高的抑制作用。我们的数据提供了对富含AMP的YE的TAS2R抑制机制的更好理解,并促进了将其用作减少食品和药物中苦味的策略。
    Yeast extracts (YEs) are used in foods because of their flavour properties and ability to reduce bitterness. The adenosine 5\'-monophosphate (AMP) found in YEs is known to decrease the bitterness of some compounds. This study aimed to investigate the ability of YEs to inhibit bitter taste receptors (TAS2Rs) using in vitro cell-based assays. A screen of TAS2Rs activated by AMP and YEs revealed that AMP and the AMP-rich YE activated more TAS2Rs. The inhibitory effect of the AMP-rich YE on seven TAS2Rs activated by bitter agonists was studied. YE reduced TAS2R activation, increased the EC50 value and decreased the maximum amplitude, demonstrating competitive and non-competitive inhibitions. Amongst the nineteen TAS2Rs tested, seven showed 40 % or greater inhibition after treatment of AMP-rich YE. Our data provide a better understanding of the TAS2R inhibition mechanism of AMP-rich YEs and promote their use as a strategy to reduce bitterness in foods and medicines.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在COVID-19大流行期间,全球教育系统实施的保护措施,尤其是强制戴口罩。随着大流行的动态变化,许多市政当局取消了这些任务,保证对这些政策变化的影响进行严格的审查。这项研究考察了解除口罩的要求对马萨诸塞州学区内COVID-19传播的影响。我们首先重复了以前使用COVID-19发病率差异(DID)模型的研究。然后,我们通过用生殖数(Rt)代替结果测量来重复DID分析,反映了可传递性。由于数据的可用性,我们估计的Rt仅衡量校内传播。我们在复制中发现了类似的结果,使用发病率,对每1000名学生中39.1例(95%CI:20.4至57.4)COVID-19病例的平均治疗效果与解除掩蔽任务相关。然而,当将结果测量替换为Rt时,我们的研究结果表明,提升面罩要求和降低Rt之间没有显著关联(ATT:0.04,95%CI:-0.09至0.18),干预后的前2周除外。此外,我们估计Rt在所有学校类型的取消口罩授权前4周低于1,在实施前建议不可持续的传播。我们的重新分析表明,从长远来看,没有证据表明学校取消口罩的要求会影响COVID-19的传播。我们的研究强调了在评估针对传播的干预措施时检查传播性结果的重要性。
    Amid the COVID-19 pandemic, education systems globally implemented protective measures, notably mandatory mask wearing. As the pandemic\'s dynamics changed, many municipalities lifted these mandates, warranting a critical examination of these policy changes\' implications. This study examines the effects of lifting mask mandates on COVID-19 transmission within Massachusetts school districts. We first replicated previous research that utilized a difference-in-difference (DID) model for COVID-19 incidence. We then repeated the DID analysis by replacing the outcome measurement with the reproductive number (Rt ), reflecting the transmissibility. Due to the data availability, the Rt we estimated only measures the within school transmission. We found a similar result in the replication using incidence with an average treatment effect on treated (ATT) of 39.1 (95% CI: 20.4 to 57.4) COVID-19 cases per 1,000 students associated with lifting masking mandates. However, when replacing the outcome measurement to Rt , our findings suggest that no significant association between lifting mask mandates and reduced Rt (ATT: 0.04, 95% CI: -0.09 to 0.18), except for the first 2 weeks postintervention. Moreover, we estimated Rt below 1 at 4 weeks before lifting mask mandates across all school types, suggesting nonsustainable transmission before the implementation. Our reanalysis suggested no evidence of lifting mask mandates in schools impacted the COVID-19 transmission in the long term. Our study highlights the importance of examining the transmissibility outcome when evaluating interventions against transmission.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    光谱特性对昼夜节律的影响对于基于实验室的昼夜节律系统研究以及对夜间人造光影响的基于场的理解具有重要意义。关于掩蔽行为的强度和光谱之间的权衡在很大程度上是未知的,即使是经过充分研究的生物体。我们使用定制的LED照明系统来记录野生型家鼠(Musmusculus)对四种强度水平(0.01、0.5、5和50lx)和三种相关色温(CCT;1750、1950和3000K)的所有组合的1小时夜间暴露的反应。较高的光强度(50lx)基本上抑制了笼子的活动,并且对于较高的CCT光始终更多(3000K为91%;1750K为53%)。在较低的强度(0.01lx),平均活动增加,最低CCT的增幅最大(1750K时增加12.3%;3000K时增加3%)。多元线性回归证实了CCT(p<0.001)和强度(p<0.001)对活性变化的影响(r2=0.66,F9,171=3.33;p<0.001),其强度大小为CCT的3.6倍。雄性小鼠的活性抑制显著低于雌性小鼠(p<0.0001)。在50lx的视交叉上核中光诱发的cFos表达的评估显示,高CCT和低CCT暴露之间没有显着差异。光谱组成的显着差异说明需要在昼夜节律行为研究中考虑光谱,并确认光谱控制可以减轻一些,但肯定不是全部,光污染对野生物种的影响。
    The influence of light spectral properties on circadian rhythms is of substantial interest to laboratory-based investigation of the circadian system and to field-based understanding of the effects of artificial light at night. The trade-offs between intensity and spectrum regarding masking behaviors are largely unknown, even for well-studied organisms. We used a custom LED illumination system to document the response of wild-type house mice (Mus musculus) to 1-h nocturnal exposure of all combinations of four intensity levels (0.01, 0.5, 5 and 50 lx) and three correlated color temperatures (CCT; 1750, 1950 and 3000 K). Higher intensities of light (50 lx) suppressed cage activity substantially, and consistently more for the higher CCT light (91% for 3000 K, 53% for 1750 K). At the lowest intensity (0.01 lx), mean activity was increased, with the greatest increases for the lowest CCT (12.3% increase at 1750 K, 3% increase at 3000 K). Multiple linear regression confirmed the influence of both CCT and intensity on changes in activity, with the scaled effect size of intensity 3.6 times greater than that of CCT. Activity suppression was significantly lower for male than for female mice. Assessment of light-evoked cFos expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus at 50 lx showed no significant difference between high and low CCT exposure. The significant differences by spectral composition illustrate a need to account for light spectrum in circadian studies of behavior, and confirm that spectral controls can mitigate some, but certainly not all, of the effects of light pollution on species in the wild.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号