Language acquisition

语言习得
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们对二元游戏过程中领导者-追随者动态塑造婴儿语言习得的机制知之甚少。我们假设婴儿决定在视觉上探索一个特定的物体信号焦点增加内源性注意力,当照顾者通过命名对象来回应这些主动行为时,它会促进婴儿的单词学习。为了检查这一点,我们邀请护理人员和他们14岁的婴儿玩新奇的东西,在测试婴儿保留新的对象-标签映射之前。同时,记录他们的脑电图。结果显示,婴儿在游戏过程中主动地注视物体,这与内源性注意力的更大神经特征有关。此外,当护理人员在这些事件中命名物体时,婴儿表现出更大的单词学习能力,但只有当护理人员也加入了他们的关注焦点时。我们的研究结果支持这样的观点,即婴儿主动视觉探索指导他们获得词典。
    We know little about the mechanisms through which leader-follower dynamics during dyadic play shape infants\' language acquisition. We hypothesized that infants\' decisions to visually explore a specific object signal focal increases in endogenous attention, and that when caregivers respond to these proactive behaviors by naming the object it boosts infants\' word learning. To examine this, we invited caregivers and their 14-mo-old infants to play with novel objects, before testing infants\' retention of the novel object-label mappings. Meanwhile, their electroencephalograms were recorded. Results showed that infants\' proactive looks toward an object during play associated with greater neural signatures of endogenous attention. Furthermore, when caregivers named objects during these episodes, infants showed greater word learning, but only when caregivers also joined their focus of attention. Our findings support the idea that infants\' proactive visual explorations guide their acquisition of a lexicon.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究人员最近认为,大型语言模型(LLM)的功能可以为有关学习和/或天生在人类语言发展和进化中的作用的长期辩论提供新的见解。这里,我们有两个理由认为,仅LLM在习得和进化方面对人类语言和认知的了解很少。首先,人类语言和LLM输出之间的任何相似之处都是纯粹的功能。借用伦理学的“四个问题”框架,我们认为LLM做的表面上是相似的,但他们是如何做到的不是。与人类在交互式语言学习中利用的丰富的多模态数据相反,LLM依赖于沉浸式暴露于大量的单峰文本数据,最近的多模式努力建立在图像和文本之间的映射之上。第二,转向人类语言和LLM输出之间的功能相似性,我们表明人类的语言行为要广泛得多。LLM的设计目的是模仿人类写作的特定行为;虽然他们这样做令人印象深刻,这些模型的潜在机制限制了它们的意义和自然主义互动的能力,以及他们应对人类语言多样性的潜力。最后,我们强调LLM不是语言理论,但是可以用来学习语言的工具,这只能有效地应用于特定的假设来激励研究。
    Researchers have recently argued that the capabilities of Large Language Models (LLMs) can provide new insights into longstanding debates about the role of learning and/or innateness in the development and evolution of human language. Here, we argue on two grounds that LLMs alone tell us very little about human language and cognition in terms of acquisition and evolution. First, any similarities between human language and the output of LLMs are purely functional. Borrowing the \"four questions\" framework from ethology, we argue that what LLMs do is superficially similar, but how they do it is not. In contrast to the rich multimodal data humans leverage in interactive language learning, LLMs rely on immersive exposure to vastly greater quantities of unimodal text data, with recent multimodal efforts built upon mappings between images and text. Second, turning to functional similarities between human language and LLM output, we show that human linguistic behavior is much broader. LLMs were designed to imitate the very specific behavior of human writing; while they do this impressively, the underlying mechanisms of these models limit their capacities for meaning and naturalistic interaction, and their potential for dealing with the diversity in human language. We conclude by emphasising that LLMs are not theories of language, but tools that may be used to study language, and that can only be effectively applied with specific hypotheses to motivate research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    否定触发的推论在人类语言中是普遍的。听到“这不是X”应该在逻辑上导致推断X以外的所有元素构成可能的替代方案。然而,并非所有逻辑上可能的替代方案在现实世界中都是平等的。作为一个合理的替代方案,它必须与否定元素尽可能多的相似之处,最合理的一个通常来自与否定元素相同的分类类别。本文报道了两个实验,这些实验调查了学龄前儿童推断由否定引发的合理替代方案的能力的发展。实验1表明,在要求儿童确定否定元素的最合理替代方案的情况下,3-,4岁和5岁的孩子,表现出对分类关联的强烈偏好。实验2进一步证明了3-,在没有明确要求他们评估不同候选人的合理性的情况下,4岁和5岁的孩子将所有补集成员视为同等可能的替代方案。一起来看,我们的发现揭示了学龄前儿童对由否定引发的合理选择做出推论的能力的有趣的发展连续性。我们讨论了潜在的语义和语用因素,这些因素有助于儿童对负面表达引发的典型替代方案的新兴意识。
    Negation-triggered inferences are universal across human languages. Hearing \"This is not X\" should logically lead to the inference that all elements other than X constitute possible alternatives. However, not all logically possible alternatives are equally accessible in the real world. To qualify as a plausible alternative, it must share with the negated element as many similarities as possible, and the most plausible one is often from the same taxonomic category as the negated element. The current article reports on two experiments that investigated the development of preschool children\'s ability to infer plausible alternatives triggered by negation. Experiment 1 showed that in a context where children were required to determine the most plausible alternative to the negated element, the 3-, 4- and 5-year-olds, exhibited a robust preference for the taxonomic associates. Experiment 2 further demonstrated that the 3-, 4- and 5-year-olds considered all the complement set members as equally possible alternatives in a context where they were not explicitly required to evaluate the plausibility of different candidates. Taken together, our findings reveal interesting developmental continuity in preschool children\'s ability to make inferences about plausible alternatives triggered by negation. We discuss the potential semantic and pragmatic factors that contribute to children\'s emerging awareness of typical alternatives triggered by negative expressions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    儿童在语言学习过程中听到的单词频率分布是高度偏斜的(Zipfian)。先前的研究表明,这种偏斜的环境在需要学习者发现必须学习的单元的任务中赋予了可学习性优势,如在分词或跨情境学习中。这种促进作用已归因于学习较低频率项目中高频项目的上下文促进,并在偏斜分布的增加的可预测性(较低的熵)下更好地学习。这里,我们问Zipfian分布是否除了发现单位之外还能促进学习,正如在可预测性账户下预期的那样。我们在一项学习任务中测试了儿童对新颖单词引用映射的学习,其中每个映射在训练期间都是孤立地呈现的,并且不需要被证明。我们将统一环境中的学习与具有不同熵水平的两个偏斜环境进行了比较。儿童的学习在两个倾斜的环境中总体上更好,即使是低频项目。这些结果将Zipfian分布的促进作用扩展到其他学习任务,并表明它们可以在发现单元之外促进语言学习。
    The word-frequency distributions children hear during language learning are highly skewed (Zipfian). Previous studies suggest that such skewed environments confer a learnability advantage in tasks that require the learner to discover the units that have to be learned, as in word-segmentation or cross-situational learning. This facilitative effect has been attributed to contextual facilitation from high frequency items in learning lower frequency items, and to better learning under the increased predictability (lower entropy) of skewed distributions. Here, we ask whether Zipfian distributions facilitate learning beyond the discovery of units, as expected under the predictability account. We tested children\'s learning of novel word-referent mappings in a learning task where each mapping was presented in isolation during training, and did not need to be dicovered. We compared learning in a uniform environment to two skewed environments with different entropy levels. Children\'s learning was overall better in the two skewed environments, even for low frequency items. These results extend the facilitative effect of Zipfian distributions to additional learning tasks and show they can facilitate language learning beyond the discovery of units.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Demonstratives(\"this\"/\"that\")表达了说话者相对距离的对比,并且需要系统地相互替换:取决于它们的相对位置,一个发言者所说的“这个”是指另一个发言者所说的“那个”。\“示范语的这种替代方面给学习示范语带来了额外的困难,因为它需要认识到两个说话者必须用不同的单词来指代同一件事,这可能是据报道长期获取示威者的原因之一。在一项用德语进行的在线研究中,调查了处于指示性获得的估计最高年龄范围(5至7岁)的儿童是否理解指示性替代方面与熟悉的(\“dies\”/\“das\”)和新颖的(\“schmi\”/\“schmu\”)指示性,以及他们是否理解非说明性地用作标签(N=73;主题之间)的新词(“schmi”/“schmu”)。儿童的准确性与成人表现进行了比较(N=74)。研究表明,5至7岁的儿童在所有情况下的表现都不如成年人准确。虽然成年人的表现在所有情况下都非常准确(正确的75%到92%),儿童在示范条件下表现低于机会,在标签条件下表现高于机会。这表明孩子们不理解所提出的设置中的指示。对儿童反应模式的更详细分析表明,他们在任何情况下都将单词视为互斥的标签。
    Demonstratives (\"this\"/\"that\") express a speaker-relative distance contrast and need to be substituted for each other systematically: depending on their relative position, what one speaker refers to by saying \"this\" another speaker has to refer to by saying \"that.\" This substitution aspect of demonstratives poses additional difficulties for learning demonstratives, because it requires recognizing that two speakers have to refer to the same thing with different words, and might be one reason for the reportedly protracted acquisition of demonstratives. In an online study conducted in German, it was investigated whether children in the estimated upper age range of demonstrative acquisition (5 to 7 years) understand demonstratives\' substitution aspect with familiar (\"dies\"/\"das\") and novel (\"schmi\"/\"schmu\") demonstratives, and whether they understand novel words (\"schmi\"/\"schmu\") when used non-demonstratively as labels (N = 73; between-subject). Children\'s accuracy was compared with adult performance (N = 74). The study shows that children between 5 and 7 years of age perform less accurately than adults in all conditions. While adults\' performance was highly accurate in all conditions (between 75% and 92% correct), children performed below chance in both demonstrative conditions and above chance in the labeling condition. This suggests that children do not understand demonstratives in the presented setup. More detailed analyses of children\'s response patterns indicate that they instead treat words as mutually exclusive labels in any condition.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一些自闭症儿童通过接触屏幕获得外语。因此,这种意想不到的双语(UB)不是由社会互动驱动的,更确切地说,语言习得似乎依赖于较少的社会中介学习和其他认知过程。我们假设UB儿童可能依赖其他线索,比如声学提示,语言输入。先前的研究表明,一些自闭症儿童的音高处理得到了加强,由于对细微的语言变化的敏感性,通常与语言延迟和难以形成稳定的语音类别有关。我们建议重复的基于屏幕的输入简化了语言的复杂性,允许专注于个人线索。这项研究假设,与自闭症和非自闭症同龄人相比,自闭症UB儿童表现出更高的音高歧视。从46名9至16岁的自闭症法语儿童的样本中,有12名被认为是UB。这些孩子,还有45名非自闭症儿童,参加了两种选择的强制选择音高歧视任务。他们听着成对的纯音,其中50%相差3%(容易),2%(中等),或1%(硬)。严格的表现比较显示,只有自闭症UB组表现高于不同音调对的机会,在所有条件下。相对于自闭症和非自闭症同龄人,该组表现出更高的音高歧视。这项研究建立了自闭症患者的UB现象,并为该人群的音高歧视提供了证据。对听觉信息的急性感知,结合重复的语言内容,可以促进UB儿童对语音特征的关注,并帮助获得没有交际支持或动机的语言。
    Some autistic children acquire foreign languages from exposure to screens. Such unexpected bilingualism (UB) is therefore not driven by social interaction, rather, language acquisition appears to rely on less socially mediated learning and other cognitive processes. We hypothesize that UB children may rely on other cues, such as acoustic cues, of the linguistic input. Previous research indicates enhanced pitch processing in some autistic children, often associated with language delays and difficulties in forming stable phonological categories due to sensitivity to subtle linguistic variations. We propose that repetitive screen-based input simplifies linguistic complexity, allowing focus on individual cues. This study hypothesizes that autistic UB children exhibit superior pitch discrimination compared with both autistic and non-autistic peers. From a sample of 46 autistic French-speaking children aged 9 to 16, 12 were considered as UB. These children, along with 45 non-autistic children, participated in a two-alternative forced-choice pitch discrimination task. They listened to pairs of pure tones, 50% of which differed by 3% (easy), 2% (medium), or 1% (hard). A stringent comparison of performance revealed that only the autistic UB group performed above chance for tone pairs that differed, across all conditions. This group demonstrated superior pitch discrimination relative to autistic and non-autistic peers. This study establishes the phenomenon of UB in autism and provides evidence for enhanced pitch discrimination in this group. Acute perception of auditory information, combined with repeated language content, may facilitate UB children\'s focus on phonetic features, and help acquire a language with no communicative support or motivation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    以前的工作表明,学龄前儿童经常误解隐喻。然而,最近的一些研究表明,学龄前儿童可以代表抽象的关系,这表明隐喻理解的认知基础可能比以前认为的更早发展。本实验使用新颖的范式来探索学龄前儿童(N=200;4-5岁;100名男性,100名女性;主要是白人)可以理解基于抽象的隐喻,功能相似性。在实验1中,学龄前儿童和成年人(N=64;18-41岁;25名男性,39名女性;主要是白人)对功能隐喻进行评级(例如,\“屋顶是帽子\”;\“轮胎是鞋子\”)比废话更“聪明”(例如,“船是裙子”;“便士是太阳镜”)在元语言判断任务中(学龄前儿童的d=.42;成人的d=3.06)。在实验2中,学龄前儿童更喜欢功能解释(例如,\“两者都让你干涸\”)而不是感性的解释(例如,“两者都有尖锐的顶部”)在解释功能隐喻时(例如,“屋顶是帽子”)(d=.99)。在实验3中,学龄前儿童更喜欢功能隐喻(例如,“屋顶是帽子”)在胡说八道的陈述上(例如,“屋顶是剪刀”),当提示选择“更好”的话语(d=1.25)时。此外,实验1中超过四分之一的学龄前儿童和实验3中一半的学龄前儿童在证明他们的反应时明确表达了功能相似性,这些儿童子集的表现推动了整个样本在两个实验中的成功。这些发现表明,学龄前儿童可以理解基于抽象的隐喻,功能相似性。
    Previous work suggests that preschoolers often misunderstand metaphors. However, some recent studies demonstrate that preschoolers can represent abstract relations, suggesting that the cognitive foundations of metaphor comprehension may develop earlier than previously believed. The present experiments used novel paradigms to explore whether preschoolers (N = 200; 4-5 years; 100 males, 100 females; predominantly White) can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities. In Experiment 1, preschoolers and adults (N = 64; 18-41 years; 25 males, 39 females; predominantly White) rated functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\"; \"Tires are shoes\") as \"smarter\" than nonsense statements (e.g., \"Boats are skirts\"; \"Pennies are sunglasses\") in a metalinguistic judgment task (d = .42 in preschoolers; d = 3.06 in adults). In Experiment 2, preschoolers preferred functional explanations (e.g., \"Both keep you dry\") over perceptual explanations (e.g., \"Both have pointy tops\") when interpreting functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\") (d = .99). In Experiment 3, preschoolers preferred functional metaphors (e.g., \"Roofs are hats\") over nonsense statements (e.g., \"Roofs are scissors\") when prompted to select the \"better\" utterance (d = 1.25). Moreover, over a quarter of preschoolers in Experiment 1 and half of preschoolers in Experiment 3 explicitly articulated functional similarities when justifying their responses, and the performance of these subsets of children drove the success of the entire sample in both experiments. These findings demonstrate that preschoolers can understand metaphors based on abstract, functional similarities.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    心理语言学和认知科学的研究人员详细记录了儿童对语言的建构主义习得。然而,尽管在人工智能领域类似人的通信是中心的,迄今为止,儿童学习语言的机制还没有忠实的计算操作。在这篇文章中,我们通过引入语法语义模式发现的建构主义语言习得的机械模型来填补部分空白。具体而言,我们提出了一种仅基于语言观察的形式和含义的异同来学习语法的方法。由此产生的语法由可变范围和抽象程度的形式意义映射组成,叫做建筑,这促进了语言的理解和生产。将我们的方法应用于CLEVR基准数据集,我们提供了一个概念证明,展示了在线,增量,数据高效,从话语意义对中透明有效地学习基于项目的结构语法。
    The constructivist acquisition of language by children has been elaborately documented by researchers in psycholinguistics and cognitive science. However, despite the centrality of human-like communication in the field of artificial intelligence, no faithful computational operationalizations of the mechanisms through which children learn language exist to date. In this article, we fill part of this void by introducing a mechanistic model of the constructivist acquisition of language through syntactico-semantic pattern finding. Concretely, we present a methodology for learning grammars based on similarities and differences in the form and meaning of linguistic observations alone. The resulting grammars consist of form-meaning mappings of variable extent and degree of abstraction, called constructions, which facilitate both language comprehension and production. Applying our methodology to the CLEVR benchmark dataset, we provide a proof of concept that demonstrates the online, incremental, data-efficient, transparent and effective learning of item-based construction grammars from utterance-meaning pairs.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    测试-重新测试可靠性-确定测量值在多个测试会话中保持一致-对于测量至关重要,理解,并预测婴儿语言发育的个体差异。然而,以前在婴儿言语感知任务中建立测量可靠性的尝试是有限的,经常使用的婴儿措施的可靠性在很大程度上是未知的。当前的研究调查了在ManyBabies1合作研究项目的背景下,在大样本(N=158)中,婴儿对婴儿定向语音的偏好高于成人定向语音的重测可靠性。要求实验室带来参与的婴儿进行第二次预约,重新测试婴儿对婴儿主导演讲的偏好。Thisapproachallowsustoestimatetest-retestreliabilityacrossthreedifferentmethodsusedtoinvestigatepreferentlistingin婴儿期:thehead-turnpreferenceprocedure,中央固定,和眼动追踪。总的来说,我们在婴儿言语偏好的测量中没有发现测试-重测可靠性的一致证据(总体r=0.09,95%CI[-0.06,0.25]).虽然增加了婴儿需要参与分析的试验数量,但显示了重测可靠性的数字增长,这也大大减少了研究的有效样本量。因此,未来对婴儿发育的研究应考虑到并非所有的实验措施都适用于评估婴儿之间的个体差异.研究重点:我们在一个大的预登记样本中(N=158)评估了婴儿对婴儿主导语音的重测可靠性。在婴儿言语偏好的测量中,没有一致的重测可靠性证据。应用更严格的标准纳入参与者可能会导致更高的测试-重测可靠性,但以样本量大幅减少为代价。依赖于稳定的个体差异的发展研究应考虑其措施的潜在可靠性。
    Test-retest reliability-establishing that measurements remain consistent across multiple testing sessions-is critical to measuring, understanding, and predicting individual differences in infant language development. However, previous attempts to establish measurement reliability in infant speech perception tasks are limited, and reliability of frequently used infant measures is largely unknown. The current study investigated the test-retest reliability of infants\' preference for infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech in a large sample (N = 158) in the context of the ManyBabies1 collaborative research project. Labs were asked to bring in participating infants for a second appointment retesting infants on their preference for infant-directed speech. This approach allowed us to estimate test-retest reliability across three different methods used to investigate preferential listening in infancy: the head-turn preference procedure, central fixation, and eye-tracking. Overall, we found no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants\' speech preference (overall r = 0.09, 95% CI [-0.06,0.25]). While increasing the number of trials that infants needed to contribute for inclusion in the analysis revealed a numeric growth in test-retest reliability, it also considerably reduced the study\'s effective sample size. Therefore, future research on infant development should take into account that not all experimental measures may be appropriate for assessing individual differences between infants. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We assessed test-retest reliability of infants\' preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech in a large pre-registered sample (N = 158). There was no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants\' speech preference. Applying stricter criteria for the inclusion of participants may lead to higher test-retest reliability, but at the cost of substantial decreases in sample size. Developmental research relying on stable individual differences should consider the underlying reliability of its measures.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    跨语言,语音以相同的时间顺序展开,受时间向前流动的约束。但是语音在空间上映射到拼写法的方式是特定于语言的,从左到右,从右到左,从上到下,在其他人中。虽然书写系统的方向会影响已知单词的视觉处理方式,尚不清楚它是否会影响新颖的正字法规律的学习和记忆。本研究在英语和希伯来语使用者的本地拼字法中测试了他们的拼字法单词所指映射任务(从左到右和从右到左书写,分别),其中单词的起始和偏移是单词身份的同等信息线索。虽然所有的人都学习了显著高于机会的拼字词指称映射,这个词最强烈的部分各不相同。英语单语者False最震惊的是与目标相同的二元语法开始的竞争箔,代表单词发作最强烈。然而,接受过本地正字法训练的希伯来语双语者在发病和抵消竞争对手的误报率之间没有差异,同样强烈地代表单词的开始和结束。重要的是,对英语单词进行测试的希伯来语双语者显示出更像英语的错误警报模式(尽管不是完全切换),这表明记忆偏见适应了遇到的文本的相反方向,同时保留了母语偏见的痕迹。这些发现表明,不同写作系统的经验会影响个人如何代表新颖的拼字词,从学习的最初阶段开始。
    Across languages, speech unfolds in the same temporal order, constrained by the forward flow of time. But the way phonology is spatially mapped onto orthography is language-specific, ranging from left-to-right, right-to-left, and top-to-bottom, among others. While the direction of writing systems influences how known words are visually processed, it is unclear whether it influences learning and memory for novel orthographic regularities. The present study tested English and Hebrew speakers on an orthographic word-referent mapping task in their native orthographies (written left-to-right and right-to-left, respectively), where the onsets and offsets of words were equally informative cues to word identity. While all individuals learned orthographic word-referent mappings significantly above chance, the parts of the word that were most strongly represented varied. English monolinguals false alarmed most to competing foils that began with the same bigram as the target, representing word onsets most strongly. However, Hebrew bilinguals trained on their native orthography showed no difference between false alarm rates to onset and offset competitors, representing the beginning and ends of words equally strongly. Importantly, Hebrew bilinguals tested on English words displayed a more English-like false alarm pattern (although not a full switch), suggesting that memory biases adapt to the opposite directionality of encountered text while retaining traces of native language biases. These findings demonstrate that experience with different writing systems influences how individuals represent novel orthographic words, starting in the earliest stages of learning.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号