Grouping

分组
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    长期以来,人们一直设想使用世界的内部模型来解释感官信号的原因。然而,这些账户历史上没有被检验过,通常需要在可能的解释空间中进行棘手的搜索。使用听觉场景作为案例研究,我们利用当代的计算工具在听觉世界的候选内部生成模型(受生态启发的音频合成器)中推断声音的解释。模型推论解释了许多经典的幻想。与传统的听觉错觉不同,该模型适用于任何声音,并为现实世界的声音混合物表现出类似人类的感知组织。刺激可计算性和可解释模型结构的结合实现了“丰富的伪造”,揭示了关于声音产生的额外假设,需要解释感知。结果表明,生成模型如何解释经典错觉和日常感官信号的感知,并说明了将它们纳入感知理论所涉及的机遇和挑战。
    Perception has long been envisioned to use an internal model of the world to explain the causes of sensory signals. However, such accounts have historically not been testable, typically requiring intractable search through the space of possible explanations. Using auditory scenes as a case study, we leveraged contemporary computational tools to infer explanations of sounds in a candidate internal generative model of the auditory world (ecologically inspired audio synthesizers). Model inferences accounted for many classic illusions. Unlike traditional accounts of auditory illusions, the model is applicable to any sound, and exhibited human-like perceptual organization for real-world sound mixtures. The combination of stimulus-computability and interpretable model structure enabled \'rich falsification\', revealing additional assumptions about sound generation needed to account for perception. The results show how generative models can account for the perception of both classic illusions and everyday sensory signals, and illustrate the opportunities and challenges involved in incorporating them into theories of perception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    基于人群的人类线粒体遗传多样性研究通常需要将线粒体DNA(mtDNA)单倍型分类为超过5400个描述的单倍群,并进一步将这些分组为层次结构更高的单倍群。这样的次级单倍群分组(例如,“宏观单倍群”)在不同的研究中有所不同,因为它们取决于样品质量,单倍群调用的技术因素,研究的目的,以及研究人员对mtDNA单倍群命名法的理解。保留历史命名法加上越来越多的新描述的mtDNA谱系导致越来越复杂和不一致的命名法,不能很好地反映系统发育。这种“混乱”为科学出版物中的错误和不一致的分组留下了空间,特别是当单倍群名称用作辅助分组的代理时,代表了科学误解的来源。在这里,我们探讨了系统发育不敏感的次级mtDNA单倍群分组的影响,以及缺乏对下游分析和遗传数据解释的标准化二级单倍群分组。我们证明,当应用不同的次级mtDNA分组时,基于频率的分析会产生不一致的结果,因此允许对相同的遗传数据进行截然不同的解释。缺乏关于如何选择适当的二级单倍群分组的指导方针和建议,这给结果的解释带来了一个问题。以及它们在不同研究中的比较和可重复性。为了减少源自任意定义的基于次要命名法的分组的偏差,我们建议,针对mtDNA单倍群命名法使用的mtDNA系统发育的未来更新还应提供定义明确和标准化的基于系统发育有意义算法的次级单倍群分组,例如“宏单倍群”,\"中群单倍群\",和“微单倍群”。理想情况下,每个二级单倍群分组水平都应提供有关不同人群历史事件的信息.可以使用TreeCluster轻松定义单倍群分组的系统发育信息水平,然后实现为haplogroup调用者,如HaploGrep3。这将促进跨研究的可重复性,为基于人群的研究提供分组标准,并减少未来研究中与单倍群命名相关的错误。
    Population-based studies of human mitochondrial genetic diversity often require the classification of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) haplotypes into more than 5400 described haplogroups, and further grouping those into hierarchically higher haplogroups. Such secondary haplogroup groupings (e.g., \"macro-haplogroups\") vary across studies, as they depend on the sample quality, technical factors of haplogroup calling, the aims of the study, and the researchers\' understanding of the mtDNA haplogroup nomenclature. Retention of historical nomenclature coupled with a growing number of newly described mtDNA lineages results in increasingly complex and inconsistent nomenclature that does not reflect phylogeny well. This \"clutter\" leaves room for grouping errors and inconsistencies across scientific publications, especially when the haplogroup names are used as a proxy for secondary groupings, and represents a source for scientific misinterpretation. Here we explore the effects of phylogenetically insensitive secondary mtDNA haplogroup groupings, and the lack of standardized secondary haplogroup groupings on downstream analyses and interpretation of genetic data. We demonstrate that frequency-based analyses produce inconsistent results when different secondary mtDNA groupings are applied, and thus allow for vastly different interpretations of the same genetic data. The lack of guidelines and recommendations on how to choose appropriate secondary haplogroup groupings presents an issue for the interpretation of results, as well as their comparison and reproducibility across studies. To reduce biases originating from arbitrarily defined secondary nomenclature-based groupings, we suggest that future updates of mtDNA phylogenies aimed for the use in mtDNA haplogroup nomenclature should also provide well-defined and standardized sets of phylogenetically meaningful algorithm-based secondary haplogroup groupings such as \"macro-haplogroups\", \"meso-haplogroups\", and \"micro-haplogroups\". Ideally, each of the secondary haplogroup grouping levels should be informative about different human population history events. Those phylogenetically informative levels of haplogroup groupings can be easily defined using TreeCluster, and then implemented into haplogroup callers such as HaploGrep3. This would foster reproducibility across studies, provide a grouping standard for population-based studies, and reduce errors associated with haplogroup nomenclatures in future studies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本手稿讨论了应用新方法方法(NAMs)进行先进纳米材料(AdNMs)的安全设计和监管风险评估的挑战。作者提出了涉及NAM的AdNM下一代风险评估框架,该框架与常规风险评估范式保持一致。这个框架是暴露驱动的,特定端点,充分利用预先存在的信息,并且可以在所采用的NAM的增加的特异性和复杂性的层次中实现。方法的分层结构,有效地将现有数据的使用与有针对性的测试相结合,将允许对安全性进行经济有效的评估,并尽可能地限制脊椎动物的使用。根据透明度评估最先进的新兴NAM的监管准备情况,可靠性,可访问性,适用性,相关性和完整性,并就风险评估范式的每个步骤讨论了它们与AdNM的相关性,并为各自科学和监管领域的未来发展提供了观点。
    This manuscript discusses the challenges of applying New Approach Methodologies (NAMs) for safe by design and regulatory risk assessment of advanced nanomaterials (AdNMs). The authors propose a framework for Next Generation Risk Assessment of AdNMs involving NAMs that is aligned to the conventional risk assessment paradigm. This framework is exposure-driven, endpoint-specific, makes best use of pre-existing information, and can be implemented in tiers of increasing specificity and complexity of the adopted NAMs. The tiered structure of the approach, which effectively combines the use of existing data with targeted testing will allow safety to be assessed cost-effectively and as far as possible with even more limited use of vertebrates. The regulatory readiness of state-of-the-art emerging NAMs is assessed in terms of Transparency, Reliability, Accessibility, Applicability, Relevance and Completeness, and their appropriateness for AdNMs is discussed in relation to each step of the risk assessment paradigm along with providing perspectives for future developments in the respective scientific and regulatory areas.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    大量研究探索了生物运动(BM)人群的显着社会意义,主要集中在均匀分布的。然而,现实世界的BM人群通常表现出分层结构,而不是统一的安排。如何处理这种结构化的BM人群仍然是一个问题。这项研究调查了结构化BM人群在工作记忆(WM)中的表现,认识到WM在我们涉及BM的社交互动中的关键作用。我们提出了基于群体的集成假设,并通过成员识别任务对其进行了测试。要求参与者辨别所呈现的BM是否属于八个BM的先前记忆显示,每个人都有不同的行走方向。借鉴突出的格式塔原则作为组织线索,我们通过在实验1和2中分别应用接近度和相似性线索,在BM人群中构建了结构化组。在实验3中,我们通过增加子集之间的相似性来故意削弱刺激结构的可见性,探索结果的稳健性。始终如一,我们的发现表明,与子集的平均方向一致的BM更有可能被认为是记忆刺激的一部分.这表明WM固有地根据组织线索将结构化的BM人群组织成单独的集合。实质上,我们的结果阐明了WM内BM人群的分组和集成编码机制的同时操作。
    Massive studies have explored biological motion (BM) crowds processing for their remarkable social significance, primarily focused on uniformly distributed ones. However, real-world BM crowds often exhibit hierarchical structures rather than uniform arrangements. How such structured BM crowds are processed remains a subject of inquiry. This study investigates the representation of structured BM crowds in working memory (WM), recognizing the pivotal role WM plays in our social interactions involving BM. We propose the group-based ensemble hypothesis and test it through a member identification task. Participants were required to discern whether a presented BM belonged to a prior memory display of eight BM, each with distinct walking directions. Drawing on prominent Gestalt principles as organizational cues, we constructed structured groups within BM crowds by applying proximity and similarity cues in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. In Experiment 3, we deliberately weakened the visibility of stimuli structures by increasing the similarity between subsets, probing the robustness of results. Consistently, our findings indicate that BM aligned with the mean direction of the subsets was more likely to be recognized as part of the memory stimuli. This suggests that WM inherently organizes structured BM crowds into separate ensembles based on organizational cues. In essence, our results illuminate the simultaneous operation of grouping and ensemble encoding mechanisms for BM crowds within WM.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    该手稿提出了一种相似性评估程序,作为对多组分纳米材料(MCNM)进行分组的基础。该方法是Zabeo等人的方法的改编。(2022),其中包括一个有影响的变化:计算的相似性在[0,1]域中通过非对称Logistic缩放进行归一化,以简化属性距离之间的比较。这种新颖的方法允许对不受数据集影响的纳米材料进行分组,这样,当新候选人被纳入评估材料集时,组成员身份不会改变。它可用于评估MCNM组以及多组分和单组分纳米材料以及化学品的混合组。为了促进拟议方法的应用,使用Python编程语言开发了一个软件脚本,它目前正在迁移到一个用户友好的基于Web的工具。所提出的方法是针对安达卢西亚可持续解决方案创新中心(CIAC)提供的真实工业案例研究进行测试的:用于建筑涂料的SiO2-ZnO混合纳米复合材料,其旨在促进从大气中光催化去除NOx气体。在案例研究中应用该方法的结果表明,ZnO与其他候选物不同,主要是由于其溶解曲线不同。
    This manuscript presents a procedure for similarity assessment as a basis for grouping of multi component nanomaterials (MCNMs). This methodology is an adaptation of the approach by Zabeo et al. (2022), which includes an impactful change: the calculated similarities are normalised in the [0,1] domain by means of asymmetric Logistic scaling to simplify comparisons among properties\' distances. This novel approach allows for grouping of nanomaterials that is not affected by the dataset, so that group membership will not change when new candidates are included in the set of assessed materials. It can be applied to assess groups of MCNMs as well as mixed groups of multi and single component nanomaterials as well as chemicals. To facilitate the application of the proposed methodology, a software script was developed by using the Python programming language, which is currently undergoing migration to a user-friendly web-based tool. The presented approach was tested against a real industrial case study provided by the Andalusian Innovation Centre for Sustainable Solution (CIAC): SiO2-ZnO hybrid nanocomposite used in building coatings, which is designed to facilitate photocatalytic removal of NOx gases from the atmosphere. The results of applying the methodology in the case study demonstrated that ZnO is dissimilar from the other candidates mainly due to its different dissolution profiles.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    阅读(RAx)和将化学品分类是毒理学中众所周知的概念。最近,ECHA提出了一种用于危险识别的支链羧酸(BCA)的分组方法,其中包括60多种支链饱和羧酸。分组仅基于结构考虑。由于两个成员的发展影响,ECHA假定“所有短碳链酸。..很可能是生殖和发育毒物。“这项工作分析了BCA的可用数据。通过消除BCA的金属和有机盐,可以显着减少该组中化合物的数量,未知或可变组成的化合物,和复杂的反应产物或生物材料(UVCB化合物)。对于所产生的减少数量的化合物,类似的物理化学数据和预期的类似生物转化支持分组。然而,对该组化合物的不良反应分析和机理信息表明,BCA,作为一个班级,不会对大鼠造成发育影响。相反,发育毒性仅限于具有共同作用模式(组蛋白脱乙酰酶抑制)的特定结构的选定BCA。因此,提议的分组范围不合理,更详细的分析表明,仅结构相似性不足以将支链羧酸分组以产生发育毒性。
    Read-across (RAx) and grouping of chemicals into categories are well-known concepts in toxicology. Recently, ECHA proposed a grouping approach for branched-chain carboxylic acids (BCAs) including more than 60 branched-chain saturated carboxylic acids for hazard identification. Grouping was based only on structural considerations. Due to developmental effects of two members, ECHA postulated that \"all short carbon chain acids … are likely reproductive and developmental toxicants\". This work analyzes available data for BCAs. The number of compounds in the group can be significantly reduced by eliminating metal and organic salts of BCAs, compounds of unknown or variable composition, and complex reaction products or biological materials (UVCB compounds). For the resulting reduced number of compounds, grouping is supported by similar physicochemical data and expected similar biotransformation. However, analysis of adverse effects for compounds in the group and mechanistic information show that BCAs, as a class, do not cause developmental effects in rats. Rather, developmental toxicity is limited to selected BCAs with specific structures that share a common mode of action (histone deacetylase inhibition). Thus, the proposed grouping is unreasonably wide and the more detailed analyses show that structural similarity alone is not sufficient for grouping branched-chain carboxylic acids for developmental toxicity.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    众所周知,当对象共享一些视觉特征时,它们会在感知组织中分组,就像一个共同的运动方向。不太为人所知的是,分组可以改变人们对一组对象的感知方式。例如,当一对形状一致共享一个共同的空间区域时,它们的长宽比往往被认为更相似(彼此吸引)。相反,当形状被分配给空间中的不同区域时,它们的纵横比彼此排斥。在这里,我们检查当一对形状之间的分组状态随时间变化时,视觉系统是否会产生吸引和排斥的扭曲。观察者观察了一对椭圆,它们的平坦程度或高度不同,并报告了这对椭圆的长宽比。每个椭圆都由一团连贯移动的点定义,两个椭圆内的点具有相同或不同的运动方向,从审判到审判。我们发现,当形状具有不同的运动方向时,与当它们具有相同的运动方向时相比,提示椭圆的纵横比被报告为与未提示椭圆的纵横比相排斥。这些结果表明,视觉系统可以根据分组自适应地改变视觉体验,特别是,当物体看起来不在一起时,排斥它们的外观,它可以快速灵活地做到这一点。
    It is well known that objects become grouped in perceptual organization when they share some visual feature, like a common direction of motion. Less well known is that grouping can change how people perceive a set of objects. For example, when a pair of shapes consistently share a common region of space, their aspect ratios tend to be perceived as more similar (are attracted toward each other). Conversely, when shapes are assigned to different regions in space their aspect ratios repel from each other. Here we examine whether the visual system produce both attractive and repulsive distortions when the state of grouping between a pair of shapes changes on a moment-to-moment basis. Observers viewed a pair of ellipses that differed in terms of how flat or tall they were and reported the aspect ratio of one ellipse from the pair. Each ellipse was defined by a cloud of coherently-moving dots, and the dots within the two ellipses had either the same or different directions of motion, varying from trial-to-trial. We found that the cued ellipse\'s aspect ratio was reported to be repelled from the aspect ratio of the uncued ellipse when the shapes had different directions of motion compared to when they had the same direction of motion. These results suggest that the visual system can adaptively alter visual experience based on grouping, in particular, repelling the appearance of objects when they do not appear to go together, and it can do so quickly and flexibly.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当记住诸如Kanizsa图形之类的集成对象时,通过对在视觉工作记忆(VWM)中呈现整体对象表示的过程进行分组,可以将部分完成为连贯的整体。本研究测量了事件相关电位(ERP)和振荡幅度,以跟踪这些编码过程并表示VWM中对象的多个特征。为此,执行了变化检测任务,这要求观察者记住六个“pacman”项目的方向和颜色,同时诱导pacmen的配置,这些配置在分组强度方面有系统地变化。尽管物理上恒定的视觉输入,但结果显示了VWM中对象配置的影响:随着分组强度的增加,方向和颜色特征的变化检测更加准确。在电生理水平上,横向化的ERP和alpha活性反映了这种行为模式。对方向特征的感知引起了对Ppc振幅所反映的分组对象的编码。分组的对象结构,反过来,如增强的N1pc和N2pc所示,调制对方向和颜色特征的关注。最后,在项目保留期间,通过分组来调制各个对象的表示以及对这些记忆对象的注意力的并发分配,正如CDA振幅的变化和同时的横向α抑制所反映的那样,分别。这些结果表明,记忆分组的多个特征,要集成的对象涉及多个,处理的顺序阶段,提供对VWM中对象表示的分层模型的支持。
    When memorizing an integrated object such as a Kanizsa figure, the completion of parts into a coherent whole is attained by grouping processes which render a whole-object representation in visual working memory (VWM). The present study measured event-related potentials (ERPs) and oscillatory amplitudes to track these processes of encoding and representing multiple features of an object in VWM. To this end, a change detection task was performed, which required observers to memorize both the orientations and colors of six \"pacman\" items while inducing configurations of the pacmen that systematically varied in terms of their grouping strength. The results revealed an effect of object configuration in VWM despite physically constant visual input: change detection for both orientation and color features was more accurate with increased grouping strength. At the electrophysiological level, the lateralized ERPs and alpha activity mirrored this behavioral pattern. Perception of the orientation features gave rise to the encoding of a grouped object as reflected by the amplitudes of the Ppc. The grouped object structure, in turn, modulated attention to both orientation and color features as indicated by the enhanced N1pc and N2pc. Finally, during item retention, the representation of individual objects and the concurrent allocation of attention to these memorized objects were modulated by grouping, as reflected by variations in the CDA amplitude and a concurrent lateralized alpha suppression, respectively. These results indicate that memorizing multiple features of grouped, to-be-integrated objects involves multiple, sequential stages of processing, providing support for a hierarchical model of object representations in VWM.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    全氟烷基和多氟烷基物质(PFAS)在商业中广泛使用,导致其在环境中普遍存在。由于其化学稳定性,PFAS被认为是持久性和生物蓄积性的;它们在环境和人类中都经常被检测到。正因为如此,PFAS作为一类(由数百至数千种化学物质组成)是非常受关注的污染物。绝大多数PFAS的信息很少,和监管机构缺乏安全数据来确定是否需要暴露限制或限制。基于细胞的检测是一种实用的方法,可以告知决策者潜在的健康危害;因此,我们假设有针对性的人体体外试验可用于确定PFAS是否存在结构-生物活性关系,并通过将生物活性(出发点)与暴露估计进行比较来表征潜在风险。我们测试了来自8个基于结构的类别的56个PFAS的浓度响应(0.1-100μM),使用从靶器官中选择的6种人类细胞类型,提示PFAS-人诱导多能干细胞(iPSC)衍生的肝细胞的不利影响,神经元,和心肌细胞,原代人肝细胞,内皮细胞和HepG2细胞。虽然许多化合物没有效果;某些PFAS表现出细胞特异性活性,突出了使用体外模型汇编来识别潜在危害的必要性。除了一些与链长和结构相关的趋势外,没有明显的特定类别分组。此外,使用经验和预测的暴露数据得出暴露裕度(MOE)。保守的MOE计算表明,大多数测试的PFAS的MOE在1-100范围内;约20%的PFAS的MOE<1,为进一步的研究提供了分层的优先级。总的来说,我们表明,基于人类细胞的模型的汇编可用于得出一系列PFAS的生物活性估计,能够与人类生物监测数据进行比较。此外,我们强调,建立结构-生物活性关系可能是具有挑战性的测试PFAS。
    Per- and poly-fluoroalkyl substances (PFAS) are extensively used in commerce leading to their prevalence in the environment. Due to their chemical stability, PFAS are considered to be persistent and bioaccumulative; they are frequently detected in both the environment and humans. Because of this, PFAS as a class (composed of hundreds to thousands of chemicals) are contaminants of very high concern. Little information is available for the vast majority of PFAS, and regulatory agencies lack safety data to determine whether exposure limits or restrictions are needed. Cell-based assays are a pragmatic approach to inform decision-makers on potential health hazards; therefore, we hypothesized that a targeted battery of human in vitro assays can be used to determine whether there are structure-bioactivity relationships for PFAS, and to characterize potential risks by comparing bioactivity (points of departure) to exposure estimates. We tested 56 PFAS from 8 structure-based subclasses in concentration response (0.1-100 μM) using six human cell types selected from target organs with suggested adverse effects of PFAS - human induced pluripotent stem cell (iPSC)-derived hepatocytes, neurons, and cardiomyocytes, primary human hepatocytes, endothelial and HepG2 cells. While many compounds were without effect; certain PFAS demonstrated cell-specific activity highlighting the necessity of using a compendium of in vitro models to identify potential hazards. No class-specific groupings were evident except for some chain length- and structure-related trends. In addition, margins of exposure (MOE) were derived using empirical and predicted exposure data. Conservative MOE calculations showed that most tested PFAS had a MOE in the 1-100 range; ∼20% of PFAS had MOE<1, providing tiered priorities for further studies. Overall, we show that a compendium of human cell-based models can be used to derive bioactivity estimates for a range of PFAS, enabling comparisons with human biomonitoring data. Furthermore, we emphasize that establishing structure-bioactivity relationships may be challenging for the tested PFAS.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当容易识别的孤立刺激被具有相似特性的刺激包围时,就会发生拥挤,很难识别。建议将拥挤作为一种机制,在对象识别和感知中造成瓶颈。最近,我们发现,短暂的呈现时间在中央凹导致对目标识别显著的拥挤效应,损害了目标的颜色意识,导致反应时间变慢。然而,用红色字母标记目标时,拥挤效应被废除。拥挤被广泛认为是一个群体;因此,它是预先集中的。调查拥挤的时空特性的事件相关电位(ERP)研究表明,涉及更高级别的视觉处理。这里,我们调查了ERP的组件是否会受到拥挤和标记的影响,以及是否可以利用ERP的时间优势来获得有关拥挤机制的进一步信息。参与者使用我们的标准中央凹鸣叫范式报告了目标识别。假设拥挤是由于抑制作用而发生的;因此,可以通过感知(N1,〜160ms)和专心(P3〜300-400ms)分量的变化来探测。我们发现在中央凹拥挤下N1有抑制作用(负ERP幅度较小),在标记条件下恢复。ERP的振幅分量(N1和P3)与行为正确比例高度相关。这些发现表明,拥挤是一种早期的分组机制,可以与涉及分割机制的后期处理相结合。
    Crowding occurs when an easily identified isolated stimulus is surrounded by stimuli with similar properties, making it very difficult to identify. Crowding is suggested as a mechanism that creates a bottleneck in object recognition and awareness. Recently, we showed that brief presentation times at the fovea resulted in a significant crowding effect on target identification, impaired the target\'s color awareness, and resulted in a slower reaction time. However, when tagging the target with a red letter, the crowding effect is abolished. Crowding is widely considered a grouping; hence, it is pre-attentive. An event-related potential (ERP) study that investigated the spatial-temporal properties of crowding suggested the involvement of higher-level visual processing. Here, we investigated whether ERP\'s components may be affected by crowding and tagging, and whether the temporal advantage of ERP can be utilized to gain further information about the crowding mechanism. The participants reported target identification using our standard foveal crowing paradigm. It is assumed that crowding occurs due to a suppressive effect; thus, it can be probed by changes in perceptual (N1, ~160 ms) and attentive (P3 ~300-400 ms) components. We found a suppression effect (less negative ERP magnitude) in N1 under foveal crowding, which was recovered under tagging conditions. ERP\'s amplitude components (N1 and P3) and the behavioral proportion correct are highly correlated. These findings suggest that crowding is an early grouping mechanism that may be combined with later processing involving the segmentation mechanism.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号