First impressions

第一印象
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    自COVID-19爆发以来,视频会议平台的使用在全球范围内有所增加,以促进面对面的交流。但是,我们在Zoom上遇到的人的第一印象与面对面互动相比如何?具体来说,我们是否认为他人的个性是准确的(符合他们独特的个性)和规范的(符合平均水平,可取的人格)作为人?在两次Zoom第一印象循环研究中(探索性研究:N=567,Dyads=3,053;预注册复制:N=371,Dyads=1,961),我们将其与面对面循环研究进行了比较(N=306;Dyads=1,682),人们在Zoom上像亲自一样准确和规范地看待他人的个性。此外,人们更喜欢他们更准确、更规范地看待的互动伙伴。然而,在音视频质量较差的互动中,人们不那么准确地看待别人,不太规范,更不喜欢他们。总的来说,通过屏幕,我们对他人的印象和面对面一样准确和规范,但这取决于屏幕的质量。
    The use of videoconferencing platforms has globally risen to facilitate face-to-face communication since the onset of COVID-19. But how do our first impressions of people we meet on Zoom compare to in-person interactions? Specifically, do we view others\' personalities as accurately (in line with their unique personality) and normatively (in line with the average, desirable personality) as in-person? Across two Zoom first-impression round-robin studies (exploratory study: N = 567, Dyads = 3,053; preregistered replication: N = 371, Dyads = 1,961), which we compared to an in-person round-robin study (N = 306; Dyads = 1,682), people viewed others\' personalities as accurately and as normatively on Zoom as in-person. Moreover, people better liked interaction partners they viewed more accurately and normatively. However, in interactions of poorer audio-video quality, people viewed others less accurately, less normatively, and liked them less. Overall, through a screen, our impressions of others are as accurate and normative as face-to-face, but it depends on the quality of that screen.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    声音引发了我们所听到的人可能是什么样的丰富的第一印象。研究强调,这些来自声音的印象是在不同的听众之间共享的,这样,人们平均同意哪些声音听起来值得信任或陈旧,哪些声音不值得。然而,来自声音的印象也可以由“旁观者的耳朵”塑造吗?我们调查了听众是否以及如何特质,个人喜好有助于声音的第一印象。在两项研究中(993名参与者,156个声音),我们使用方差分配方法找到了对语音印象的大量特质贡献的证据。总的来说,特质贡献与声音对推断的人物特征的印象的共同贡献一样重要(例如,可信度,友好)。共享贡献仅对更直接明显的人物特征的印象更具影响力(例如,性别,年龄)。当刺激的(感知的)变异性受到限制时,特质和共同的贡献都会减少,这表明声音的自然变化是理解这种印象形成的关键。当比较语音印象和脸部印象时,我们发现,当刺激属性紧密匹配时,不同模态对印象的特质和共同贡献相似-尽管语音印象总体上不如面部印象一致。因此,我们将声音中的印象重新概念化为不仅基于共同的贡献,而且基于特质的贡献而形成。我们用这个新的框架来建议未来的研究方向,包括了解特殊机制,发展,和语音印象形成的延展性。
    Voices elicit rich first impressions of what the person we are hearing might be like. Research stresses that these impressions from voices are shared across different listeners, such that people on average agree which voices sound trustworthy or old and which do not. However, can impressions from voices also be shaped by the \'ear of the beholder\'? We investigated whether - and how - listeners\' idiosyncratic, personal preferences contribute to first impressions from voices. In two studies (993 participants, 156 voices), we find evidence for substantial idiosyncratic contributions to voice impressions using a variance portioning approach. Overall, idiosyncratic contributions were as important as shared contributions to impressions from voices for inferred person characteristics (e.g., trustworthiness, friendliness). Shared contributions were only more influential for impressions of more directly apparent person characteristics (e.g., gender, age). Both idiosyncratic and shared contributions were reduced when stimuli were limited in their (perceived) variability, suggesting that natural variation in voices is key to understanding this impression formation. When comparing voice impressions to face impressions, we found that idiosyncratic and shared contributions to impressions similarly across modality when stimulus properties are closely matched - although voice impressions were overall less consistent than face impressions. We thus reconceptualise impressions from voices as being formed not only based on shared but also idiosyncratic contributions. We use this new framing to suggest future directions of research, including understanding idiosyncratic mechanisms, development, and malleability of voice impression formation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    可信度的印象很快就会从面孔中形成。相同或不同种族的观察者之间在多大程度上共享这些印象?尽管一直有关于可信度评估的高度共识的报道,最近的研究表明,个体差异很大。例如,负面的隐性种族偏见和对其他种族的个人的低接触经验已被证明与其他种族面孔的低可信度判断有关。这项预先注册的研究进一步研究了隐含的社会偏见和经验对其他种族面孔的可信度判断的影响。白人(N=338)和黑人(N=299)参与者的相对较大样本完成了三项任务:人脸的可信度评级任务,种族内隐联想测验,和一份经验问卷。每位参与者对100张白脸和100张黑脸的可信度进行了评分。我们发现,其他种族面孔的整体可信度受到内隐偏见和其他种族个人经验的影响。尽管如此,当与自己的种族基线评级进行比较时,对于具有不同水平的内隐偏见和经验的参与者,其他种族面孔的可信度等级的相对差异观察到高度相关.这些结果表明社会概念的不同影响(例如,隐性偏见,经验)与本能(例如,方法与避免的决定)关于可信度印象的决定,如总体与其他种族面孔的相对评级。
    Impressions of trustworthiness are formed quickly from faces. To what extent are these impressions shared among observers of the same or different races? Although high consensus of trustworthiness evaluation has been consistently reported, recent studies suggested substantial individual differences. For instance, negative implicit racial bias and low contact experience towards individuals of the other race have been shown to be related to low trustworthiness judgments for other-race faces. This pre-registered study further examined the effects of implicit social bias and experience on trustworthiness judgments of other-race faces. A relatively large sample of White (N = 338) and Black (N = 299) participants completed three tasks: a trustworthiness rating task of faces, a race implicit association test, and a questionnaire of experience. Each participant rated trustworthiness of 100 White faces and 100 Black faces. We found that the overall trustworthiness ratings for other-race faces were influenced by both implicit bias and experience with individuals of the other-race. Nonetheless, when comparing to the own-race baseline ratings, high correlations were observed for the relative differences in trustworthiness ratings of other-race faces for participants with varied levels of implicit bias and experience. These results suggest differential impact of social concepts (e.g., implicit bias, experience) vs. instinct (e.g., decision of approach-vs-avoid) on trustworthiness impressions, as revealed by overall vs. relative ratings on other-race faces.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当孩子们被明确询问哪张脸显得漂亮或刻薄时,他们会从面孔中推断出人格特质。关于儿童如何隐含地使用面部特征信息进行行为评估的知识很少。我们使用歧义情况协议来探索当目标儿童的行为或意图不清楚时,儿童如何使用面部特征信息来形成对歧义情况的解释。在每次审判中,儿童(N=144,年龄范围=4-11.95岁;74名女孩,67个男孩,未指定3个性别;70%白人,10%的其他种族或混血儿,5%亚洲人,4%黑色,1%土著,9%未指定)在看到孩子的面部嵌入模糊场景(场景任务)或听到关于该孩子的不当行为的小插图(不当行为任务)之前,查看了孩子的面部(先前评级为高或低)。孩子们描述了每个场景中发生的事情,并指出每个不当行为是故意还是偶然的。孩子们还评估了每个孩子的行为,并指出孩子是否会成为好朋友。面部和善影响了4岁儿童对模棱两可的行为(场景任务)的解释,6年和模棱两可的意图(不当行为任务)。我们的结果表明,使用面部特征线索来形成对模棱两可的行为的解释出现在儿童早期,这种偏见可能会导致对高-好的脸与低-好的脸的同龄人的区别对待。
    Children infer personality traits from faces when they are asked explicitly which face appears nice or mean. Less is known about how children use face-trait information implicitly to make behavioral evaluations. We used the Ambiguous Situations Protocol to explore how children use face-trait information to form interpretations of ambiguous situations when the behavior or intention of the target child was unclear. On each trial, children (N = 144, age range = 4-11.95 years; 74 girls, 67 boys, 3 gender not specified; 70% White, 10% other or mixed race, 5% Asian, 4% Black, 1% Indigenous, 9% not specified) viewed a child\'s face (previously rated high or low in niceness) before seeing the child\'s face embedded within an ambiguous scene (Scene Task) or hearing a vignette about a misbehavior done by that child (Misbehavior Task). Children described what was happening in each scene and indicated whether each misbehavior was done on purpose or by accident. Children also rated the behavior of each child and indicated whether the child would be a good friend. Facial niceness influenced children\'s interpretations of ambiguous behavior (Scene Task) by 4 years of age, and ambiguous intentions (Misbehavior Task) by 6 years. Our results suggest that the use of face-trait cues to form interpretations of ambiguous behavior emerges early in childhood, a bias that may lead to differential treatment for peers perceived with a high-nice face versus a low-nice face.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    当听众听到一个声音时,他们很快就形成了一个复杂的第一印象,那就是那个声音背后的人可能是谁。我们使用脑电图和代表性相似性分析来描述这些来自声音的多变量第一印象是如何随着时间的推移在不同的抽象级别上出现的。我们发现,对于八个感知的身体(性别,年龄,和健康),特质(吸引力,支配地位,和可信度),和社会特征(教育程度和专业精神),表征出现得较早(刺激发作后~80毫秒),语音声学在~100毫秒到400毫秒之间为这些表示做出贡献。虽然人的特征的印象是高度相关的,我们可以找到高度抽象的证据,个体特征的独立表示。这些抽象的表示随着时间的推移逐渐合并。也就是说,身体特征的表示(年龄,性别)早期出现(从~120毫秒),而一些特质和社会特征的表征稍后出现(~360毫秒以上)。这些发现与最近的理论模型一致,并阐明了支持人从声音感知的计算。
    When listeners hear a voice, they rapidly form a complex first impression of who the person behind that voice might be. We characterize how these multivariate first impressions from voices emerge over time across different levels of abstraction using electroencephalography and representational similarity analysis. We find that for eight perceived physical (gender, age, and health), trait (attractiveness, dominance, and trustworthiness), and social characteristics (educatedness and professionalism), representations emerge early (~80 ms after stimulus onset), with voice acoustics contributing to those representations between ~100 ms and 400 ms. While impressions of person characteristics are highly correlated, we can find evidence for highly abstracted, independent representations of individual person characteristics. These abstracted representationse merge gradually over time. That is, representations of physical characteristics (age, gender) arise early (from ~120 ms), while representations of some trait and social characteristics emerge later (~360 ms onward). The findings align with recent theoretical models and shed light on the computations underpinning person perception from voices.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    第一印象会影响基于评分者的判断,但它们对评分者偏见的贡献尚不清楚。研究表明,评分者可以在实验考试环境中克服第一印象,并带有明确的第一印象,但是这些发现可能无法推广到具有隐含第一印象的工作场所环境。这项研究有两个目的。首先,当工作场所表现发生变化时,评估第一印象是否会影响评估者的判断。第二,与隐含形成的第一印象相比,明确陈述这些印象是否会影响后续评级。医师评估者观看了六个视频,其中学习者的表现发生了变化(强到弱或弱到强)或保持一致。评估者被分配两组。第一组(n=23,显式)做出了第一印象的全球评级(FIGR),然后使用Mini-CEX对学习者进行评分。第二组(n=22,隐式)在视频末尾仅使用Mini-CEX对学习者进行了评分。对于“显式”组,在强到弱的条件下,FIGR(M=5.94)高于Mini-CEX全球评级(GR)(M=3.02,p<.001)。在弱到强的条件下,FIGR(M=2.44)低于Mini-CEXGR(M=3.96p<.001)。在一致的条件下,FIGR和Mini-CEXGR之间没有差异(M=6.61,M=6.65,p=.84)。当比较两组Mini-CEXGR时,在任何情况下都没有统计学上的显着差异。因此,评分者根据学习者的表现调整了他们的判断。此外,明确表达第一印象的评估者与遵循更自然的评估者表现出类似的评估者偏见。
    First impressions can influence rater-based judgments but their contribution to rater bias is unclear. Research suggests raters can overcome first impressions in experimental exam contexts with explicit first impressions, but these findings may not generalize to a workplace context with implicit first impressions. The study had two aims. First, to assess if first impressions affect raters\' judgments when workplace performance changes. Second, whether explicitly stating these impressions affects subsequent ratings compared to implicitly-formed first impressions. Physician raters viewed six videos where learner performance either changed (Strong to Weak or Weak to Strong) or remained consistent. Raters were assigned two groups. Group one (n = 23, Explicit) made a first impression global rating (FIGR), then scored learners using the Mini-CEX. Group two (n = 22, Implicit) scored learners at the end of the video solely with the Mini-CEX. For the Explicit group, in the Strong to Weak condition, the FIGR (M = 5.94) was higher than the Mini-CEX Global rating (GR) (M = 3.02, p < .001). In the Weak to Strong condition, the FIGR (M = 2.44) was lower than the Mini-CEX GR (M = 3.96 p < .001). There was no difference between the FIGR and the Mini-CEX GR in the consistent condition (M = 6.61, M = 6.65 respectively, p = .84). There were no statistically significant differences in any of the conditions when comparing both groups\' Mini-CEX GR. Therefore, raters adjusted their judgments based on the learners\' performances. Furthermore, raters who made their first impressions explicit showed similar rater bias to raters who followed a more naturalistic process.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    尽管近年来自闭症的包容性和接受度有所增加,自闭症患者继续面临污名化,排除,和受害。根据简短的10秒视频,在特征和行为意图方面,非自闭症成年人对自闭症成年人的评价低于非自闭症成年人。在目前的研究中,我们扩展了这一范式,以调查非自闭症成人评估者对自闭症和非自闭症儿童的第一印象,并研究评估者自身特征与对自闭症儿童偏见之间的关系。
    对来自15名自闭症儿童和15名非自闭症儿童的视频录制访谈片段,以音频和视频形式向346名本科生展示,仅音频,仅限视频,抄本,抄本或静止图像条件。参与者对每个孩子的一系列特征和行为意图进行了评估,然后完成了一系列衡量自己社交能力的问卷,自闭症特征,自闭症患者过去经历的数量和质量,和明显的自闭症耻辱。
    总的来说,自闭症儿童比非自闭症儿童受到负面评价,特别是在包含音频的条件下。具有较高社交能力和明显自闭症污名的评估者对自闭症儿童的评价更为负面,而具有更多自闭症特征和自闭症患者更积极的过去经历的评估者对自闭症儿童的评估更积极。
    这些快速的负面判断可能会导致自闭症儿童遭受的社会排斥。研究结果表明,某些个人特征可能与自闭症的污名化观点以及与自闭症患者互动的意愿下降有关。讨论了这些发现对自闭症患者的社会包容和福祉的影响。
    UNASSIGNED: Although autism inclusion and acceptance has increased in recent years, autistic people continue to face stigmatization, exclusion, and victimization. Based on brief 10-second videos, non-autistic adults rate autistic adults less favourably than they rate non-autistic adults in terms of traits and behavioural intentions. In the current study, we extended this paradigm to investigate the first impressions of autistic and non-autistic children by non-autistic adult raters and examined the relationship between the rater\'s own characteristics and bias against autistic children.
    UNASSIGNED: Segments of video recorded interviews from 15 autistic and 15 non-autistic children were shown to 346 undergraduate students in audio with video, audio only, video only, transcript, or still image conditions. Participants rated each child on a series of traits and behavioural intentions toward the child, and then completed a series of questionnaires measuring their own social competence, autistic traits, quantity and quality of past experiences with autistic people, and explicit autism stigma.
    UNASSIGNED: Overall, autistic children were rated more negatively than non-autistic children, particularly in conditions containing audio. Raters with higher social competence and explicit autism stigma rated autistic children more negatively, whereas raters with more autistic traits and more positive past experiences with autistic people rated autistic children more positively.
    UNASSIGNED: These rapid negative judgments may contribute to the social exclusion experienced by autistic children. The findings indicate that certain personal characteristics may be related to more stigmatised views of autism and decreased willingness to interact with the autistic person. The implications of the findings are discussed in relation to the social inclusion and well-being of autistic people.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    一个普遍的断言是,基于突出的性格特征,第一印象是自发地从脸上提取的。具体来说,仅仅接触一个人就足以触发核心人格特征的非自愿提取(例如,可信度,支配地位,能力),支持一系列重大判断的结果(例如,hiring,投资,electing).但事实是这样吗?注意到现有文献中的歧义,在这里,我们使用了一个重复启动程序来探索优势的印象在多大程度上从没有以这种方式评估刺激的指示的面孔中提取出来。在五个实验中,参与者的兴趣特征越来越明显(Expts。1-3)或注意力明确指向面部以产生低级/高级判断(Expts。4&5),没有观察到自发提取第一印象的证据.相反,只有当支配地位的判断是手头任务的明确要求时,启动才会出现。因此,至少使用启动方法,当前的研究结果质疑第一印象是人的感知的强制性产物的观点。
    A common assertion is that, based around prominent character traits, first impressions are spontaneously extracted from faces. Specifically, mere exposure to a person is sufficient to trigger the involuntary extraction of core personality characteristics (e.g., trustworthiness, dominance, competence), an outcome that supports a range of significant judgments (e.g., hiring, investing, electing). But is this in fact the case? Noting ambiguities in the extant literature, here we used a repetition priming procedure to probe the extent to which impressions of dominance are extracted from faces absent the instruction to evaluate the stimuli in this way. Across five experiments in which either the character trait of interest was made increasingly obvious to participants (Expts. 1-3) or attention was explicitly directed toward the faces to generate low-level/high-level judgments (Expts. 4 & 5), no evidence for the spontaneous extraction of first impressions was observed. Instead, priming only emerged when judgments of dominance were an explicit requirement of the task at hand. Thus, at least using a priming methodology, the current findings contest the notion that first impressions are a mandatory product of person perception.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    除了经典的免疫系统,我们已经进化出一种行为,引导我们远离潜在的传染性个体。在这里我展示,使用公开的跨文化数据,这种适应是如此的基础,以至于我们对男性陌生人的第一印象很大程度上是由他面部的健康感驱动的。积极(可爱,有能力,智能,值得信赖)和消极(不友好,无知,懒惰)第一印象以不同的方式受到面部健康的影响,与光晕效应不一致;它们还受到一个人目前的健康状态和对病原体感到厌恶的倾向的调节。这些发现,在美国和印度两个不同的国家复制,这表明,从面孔对不良和善良的本能感知不是同一枚硬币的两面,而是反映了互动的(非对称)预期成本和收益。显然,病原体进行了展示-第一印象排在第二位。我们对陌生人的第一印象(无论他们看起来是否值得信任,智能,不友好,或侵略性)是由他们的脸看起来多么健康以及我们避免感染的无意识动机决定的。坏的和好的印象结果反映了具体的,潜在的重要性,与我们的同胞互动的预期成本和收益。显然,病原体进行了展示-第一印象排在第二位。
    Along with a classical immune system, we have evolved a behavioral one that directs us away from potentially contagious individuals. Here I show, using publicly available cross-cultural data, that this adaptation is so fundamental that our first impressions of a male stranger are largely driven by the perceived health of his face. Positive (likeable, capable, intelligent, trustworthy) and negative (unfriendly, ignorant, lazy) first impressions are affected by facial health in adaptively different ways, inconsistent with a mere halo effect; they are also modulated by one\'s current state of health and inclination to feel disgusted by pathogens. These findings, which replicated across two countries as different as the USA and India, suggest that instinctive perceptions of badness and goodness from faces are not two sides of the same coin but reflect the (nonsymmetrical) expected costs and benefits of interaction. Apparently, pathogens run the show-and first impressions come second. Lay Summary: Our first impressions of strangers (whether they seem trustworthy, intelligent, unfriendly, or aggressive) are shaped by how healthy their faces look and by our unconscious motivation to avoid infections. Bad and good impressions turn out to reflect the concrete, potentially vital, expected costs and benefits of interacting with our fellow humans. Apparently, pathogens run the show-and first impressions come second.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    我们对目标文章“从面孔中理解特质印象”的六篇评论做出了回应。作者强调增加面孔和参与者多样性的重要性,达成了广泛的共识。整合对脸上印象的研究,并继续开发数据驱动方法所需的方法。我们根据这些主题提出了该领域的未来方向。
    We offer a response to six commentaries on our target article \'Understanding trait impressions from faces\'. A broad consensus emerged with authors emphasizing the importance of increasing the diversity of faces and participants, integrating research on impressions beyond the face, and continuing to develop methods needed for data-driven approaches. We propose future directions for the field based on these themes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号