Early learning

早期学习
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    加拿大的早期学习和儿童保育(ELCC)环境遵循概述食品规定的营养标准,许多人还鼓励反应灵敏的喂养方式,这有助于为儿童创造一个支持性的环境。看护者对儿童调节自身摄入量的能力缺乏信心,或者那些对用餐时间感到压力的人,可能会在不知情的情况下从事反应迟钝的喂养行为。庆祝喂养方法是一个灵活的框架,在行为改变理论的推动下,这建立在ELCC环境中响应式馈电的先前定义和概念的基础上。通过这种方法,有一个有意的重点是支持早期儿童教育者实施更敏感的喂养做法。该方法结合了13种目标教育者行为,这些行为与三个重叠的CELEBRATE语言类别有关,庆祝用餐时间,和庆祝游戏。这些做法承认和支持儿童的自主意识的发展,信心,和自我调节,不仅在用餐时,而且通过基于游戏的探索和语言,在食物和喂养周围全天使用。目标是让孩子们对各种各样的食物开放,培养他们的自我调节能力,并为他们一生中与食物的积极关系奠定基础。
    Early learning and child care (ELCC) settings in Canada follow nutrition standards that outline food provisions, with many also encouraging responsive feeding practices that help to create a supportive environment for children. Caregivers who lack confidence in children\'s ability to regulate their own intake, or those who feel stressed about mealtime, may unknowingly engage in less responsive feeding practices. The CELEBRATE Feeding Approach is a flexible framework, driven by behaviour change theory, that builds on previous definitions and concepts of responsive feeding in ELCC environments. Through this approach, there is an intentional focus on supporting early childhood educators to implement feeding practices that are more responsive. The approach incorporates 13 target educator behaviours related to the three overlapping categories of CELEBRATE language, CELEBRATE Mealtime, and CELEBRATE Play. These practices recognize and support the development of a child\'s sense of autonomy, confidence, and self-regulation not only at mealtimes but also through play-based exploration and language that is used throughout the day around food and feeding. The goal is that children will be open to a wide variety of food, develop their self-regulation skills, and build the foundation for a positive relationship with food throughout their lifetime.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    非正式学习空间为补充正式学习经验提供了成熟的机会。在本文中,我们提供了一种新的方法来为公共空间创造丰富的学习活动,反映了植根于发展心理学的循证实践,并使用参与式研究方法中的社区中心实践。我们首先认为,现有的理论和研究支持使用引导游戏教学法来促进学习。第二,我们认为,将研究有效地转化为实践应该在设计的每个阶段都融入社区的声音,实施和评估过程。我们描述了一项名为PlayfulLearningLandscapes的新举措,该举措反映了核心发展理论的宗旨,包括建构主义和社会学习理论以及指导性游戏教学法。顽皮的学习景观还通过与社区合作为当地社区空间设计活动,扩展了这些证据支持的理论的范围和规模。一起来看,我们提供了一种公平地坚持核心发展理论的方法,文化包容性研究和干预实践。将社区空间转变为儿童学习的中心有望对公平获取产生深远的影响,学校准备和幼儿教育。
    Informal learning spaces present ripe opportunities to supplement formal learning experiences. In this paper, we offer a new approach to creating enriching learning activities for public spaces that reflects evidence-based practices rooted in developmental psychology and uses community-centring practices from participatory research approaches. We first argue that extant theory and research supports the use of guided play pedagogy to foster learning. Second, we argue that effective translation of research to practice should incorporate community voices at every stage of the design, implementation and evaluation process. We describe a new initiative called Playful Learning Landscapes that reflects tenets of core developmental theory including constructivism and social learning theories as well as guided play pedagogy. Playful Learning Landscapes also extends the scope and scale of these evidence-backed theories by collaborating with communities to design activities for local community spaces. Taken together, we offer a way of upholding core developmental theory with equitable, culturally inclusive research and intervention practices. Transforming community spaces into hubs for children\'s learning promises wide-reaching implications for equitable access, school readiness and early childhood education.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:本研究评估了加纳北部在营养和健康服务以及社区平台中整合响应式护理和早期学习(RCEL)后护理人员实践的变化。
    方法:我们对医疗机构工作人员和社区卫生志愿者进行了培训,通过现有的医疗机构和社区团体为2岁以下儿童的照顾者提供RCEL咨询。我们通过家庭问卷和照顾者-儿童观察评估了干预前后照顾者RCEL实践的变化。
    方法:这项研究在萨格纳里古进行,Gushegu,Wa东,以及2022年4月至2023年3月的Mamprugu-Moagduri地区。研究地点包括加纳卫生服务设施的79个儿童福利诊所(CWC)和80个村庄储蓄和贷款协会团体(VSLA)。
    方法:我们在研究中心招募了211名在基线时有0-23个月儿童的成人照顾者,并加入了CWC或VSLA。
    结果:我们观察到RCEL和婴幼儿喂养方法的改善,早期学习的机会(例如,在家庭环境中获取书籍和玩具),减少父母的压力。
    结论:本研究证明了将RCEL含量纳入现有营养和健康服务的有效性。这些发现可以用来发展,增强,并倡导将RCEL整合到加纳现有服务和平台的政策。未来的研究可能会探讨照顾者行为的积极变化与儿童发育结果的改善之间的关系,以及加强父亲参与护理实践的策略。改善儿童监督,并确保有利的环境。
    OBJECTIVE: This study assesses change in caregiver practices after integrating responsive care and early learning (RCEL) in nutrition and health services and community platforms in northern Ghana.
    METHODS: We trained health facility workers and community health volunteers to deliver RCEL counselling to caregivers of children under 2 years of age through existing health facilities and community groups. We assessed changes in caregivers\' RCEL practices before and after the intervention with a household questionnaire and caregiver-child observations.
    METHODS: The study took place in Sagnarigu, Gushegu, Wa East and Mamprugu-Moagduri districts from April 2022 to March 2023. Study sites included seventy-nine child welfare clinics (CWC) at Ghana Health Service facilities and eighty village savings and loan association (VSLA) groups.
    METHODS: We enrolled 211 adult caregivers in the study sites who had children 0-23 months at baseline and were enrolled in a CWC or a VSLA.
    RESULTS: We observed improvements in RCEL and infant and young child feeding practices, opportunities for early learning (e.g. access to books and playthings) in the home environment and reductions in parental stress.
    CONCLUSIONS: This study demonstrates the effectiveness of integrating RCEL content into existing nutrition and health services. The findings can be used to develop, enhance and advocate for policies integrating RCEL into existing services and platforms in Ghana. Future research may explore the relationship between positive changes in caregiver behaviour and improvements in child development outcomes as well as strategies for enhancing paternal engagement in care practices, improving child supervision and ensuring an enabling environment.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:尽管过去十年儿童生存率有了很大改善,低收入和中等收入国家(LMICs)的许多儿童仍然面临因营养不良而无法发挥其发育潜能的风险,健康状况不佳,缺乏刺激。产妇的参与和刺激已被确定为儿童健康发展的一些最关键的投入。然而,在撒哈拉以南非洲(SSA),关于产妇刺激与儿童发育之间联系的证据相对较少。本文旨在确定肯尼亚和赞比亚的产妇刺激与儿童发育之间的关联。以及在这些环境中最能预测发展结果的活动。
    方法:我们使用来自肯尼亚和赞比亚的前瞻性研究数据进行了描述性研究。该研究包括三轮数据收集。儿童在第一轮平均10个月大,第二轮25个月大,在第三轮中36个月大。感兴趣的主要暴露变量是母体刺激活动,我们把它归类为认知,语言,电机,和社会情感活动。感兴趣的结果是通过第三版年龄和阶段问卷(ASQ-3)测量的儿童发育。在三轮调查中,使用线性回归模型来估计总体母体刺激和特定领域母体刺激与儿童发育之间的关联。
    结果:在校正其他混杂因素后,较高的母体刺激评分与较高的ASQ评分相关(效应大小=0.25;95%CI:0.19,0.31)。对于特定领域和儿童发展(ASQ分数),语言刺激的效应大小(ES)最大(ES=0.15),而社会情绪领域活动的关联最弱(ES=-0.05).总体母体刺激与粗大运动发育最相关(ES=0.21),与解决问题的关系最小(ES=0.16)。
    结论:我们的研究结果表明,在贫困农村地区的社区中,母亲的刺激活动与儿童的发育结果之间存在很强的正相关。
    背景:NA(非临床试验)。
    Despite major improvements in child survival over the past decade, many children in low and middle-income countries (LMICs) remain at risk of not reaching their developmental potential due to malnutrition, poor health, and a lack of stimulation. Maternal engagement and stimulation have been identified as some of the most critical inputs for healthy development of children. However, relatively little evidence exists on the links between maternal stimulation and child development exists in sub-Saharan Africa (SSA). This current paper aims to identify the associations between maternal stimulation and child development in Kenya and Zambia, as well as the activities that are most predictive of developmental outcomes in these settings.
    We conducted a descriptive study using data from a prospective study in Kenya and Zambia. The study included three rounds of data collection. Children were on average 10 months old in round one, 25 months old in round two, and 36 months old in round three. The primary exposure variable of interest was maternal stimulation activities, which we grouped into cognitive, language, motor, and socio-emotional activities. The outcome of interest was child development measured through the Third Edition of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-3). Linear regression models were used to estimate the associations between overall maternal stimulation and domain-specific maternal stimulation and child development across the three rounds of the survey.
    Higher maternal stimulation scores were associated with higher ASQ scores (effect size = 0.25; 95% CI: 0.19, 0.31) after adjusting for other confounders. For domain specific and child development (ASQ scores), the largest effect size (ES) was found for language stimulation (ES = 0.15) while weakest associations were found for socio-emotional domain activities (ES= -0.05). Overall maternal stimulation was most strongly associated with gross motor development (ES = 0.21) and the least associated with problem-solving (ES = 0.16).
    Our study findings suggest a strong positive link between maternal stimulation activities and children\'s developmental outcomes among communities in poor rural settings.
    NA (not a clinical trial).
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    与COVID-19相关的封锁和幼儿园关闭导致许多幼儿将所有时间都花在家里。一些父母不得不在家工作时照顾孩子,需求的增加可能导致他们承受相当大的压力。有证据表明,在有年幼子女的父母中,那些已经存在心理和身体状况的人比其他父母适应得更差。我们考虑了父母福祉与幼儿家庭学习环境之间的关联。
    我们利用了具有全国代表性的中国家庭小组研究的数据。我们分析了大流行之前(2018年)和期间(2020年)收集的纵向数据。参与者是1,155名学龄前儿童(2020年3-5岁)的父母。进行了适度的调解模式。母亲和父亲的心理健康,抑郁症,身体健康,2018年和2020年的身体疾病是预测因素。2020年婚姻和代际冲突的频率是调解人。2020年主要照顾者报告的家庭学习活动和家庭教育支出以及父母报告的儿童保育时间是结果变量。2020年评估前3个月,各省的COVID-19病例数是主持人。孩子,父母,家庭特征和城市化是协变量。
    控制协变量,父母心理健康的改善预示着更多的家庭学习活动,父亲抑郁的增加预示着父亲在育儿上花费的时间减少。母亲身体健康的负面变化预示着家庭教育支出减少,母亲花在育儿上的时间更多。家庭冲突介导了2018年孕产妇身体疾病与家庭教育支出之间的关联。某省(i)的COVID-19病例数与母亲花更多时间在育儿上呈正相关,(ii)缓和了改善产妇身体健康和母亲在育儿上花费更少的时间的联系,(iii)缓和了家庭冲突和更多家庭教育支出的联系。
    研究结果表明,父母的心理和身体健康下降预示着家庭早期学习和护理的货币和非货币投资减少。区域大流行风险破坏了孕产妇在早期学习和护理方面的投资,特别是对于那些预先存在的物理条件。
    UNASSIGNED: COVID-19-related lockdowns and preschool closures resulted in many young children spending all their time at home. Some parents had to manage child care while working from home, and increased demands may have led them to experience considerable stress. Evidence indicates that among parents with young children, those who had pre-existing mental and physical conditions adapted less well than other parents. We considered associations between parental well-being and the home learning environment for young children.
    UNASSIGNED: We leveraged data from the nationally representative China Family Panel Studies. We analyzed longitudinal data collected before (2018) and during (2020) the pandemic. Participants were parents of 1,155 preschoolers (aged 3-5 years in 2020). Moderated mediation models were conducted. Maternal and paternal psychological well-being, depression, physical health, and physical illness in 2018 and 2020 were predictors. The frequency of marital and intergenerational conflicts in 2020 were mediators. Primary caregiver-reported engagement in home learning activities and family educational expenditure and parent-reported time spent on child care in 2020 were outcome variables. The number of COVID-19 cases in each province 3 months before the 2020 assessment was the moderator. Child, parental, and household characteristics and urbanicity were covariates.
    UNASSIGNED: Controlling for covariates, improvements in parental psychological well-being predicted more home learning activities and increases in paternal depression predicted less time spent by fathers on child care. Negative changes in maternal physical health predicted less family educational expenditure and mothers spending more time on child care. Family conflicts mediated the association between maternal physical illness in 2018 and family educational expenditure. The number of COVID-19 cases in a province (i) was positively associated with mothers spending more time on child care, (ii) moderated the association of improvements in maternal physical health and mothers spending less time on child care, and (iii) moderated the association of family conflicts and more family educational expenditure.
    UNASSIGNED: The findings indicate that decreased parental psychological and physical well-being foretells reductions in monetary and non-monetary investment in early learning and care at home. Regional pandemic risk undermines maternal investment in early learning and care, especially for those with pre-existing physical conditions.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    婴儿花费大量时间探索物体(Herzberg等人。,2021),对象探索与各个领域的学习和发展联系在一起(例如,社会,认知,马达)。但是探索首先是如何发展的,以及如何,确切地说,探索促进学习吗?解决这些面向过程的问题的一种方法是采用发展级联的观点,它认为,新的技能是从早期开发的技能中出现的,随着时间的推移,与人和物体的各种互动积累起来,影响多个发展领域(Masten&Cicchetti,2010).在这一章中,我们从发展级联的角度描述对象探索。在第2节中,我们描述了探索行为的典型和非典型轨迹,注意这些行为是如何从早期发展的认知和运动技能中产生的。在第3节中,我们讨论了对象探索如何为新型的学习机会打开大门。在第4节中,我们讨论了可能影响对象探索发展的早期经验。总之,我们的目标是传达探索技能的新发展是早期发展技能的延伸,以及看似微不足道的探索行为(例如,摇晃拨浪鼓)可能会对发育中的婴儿造成许多不同的后果。
    Infants spend much of their time exploring objects (Herzberg et al., 2021), and object exploration is linked to learning and development in various domains (e.g., social, cognitive, motor). But how does exploration develop in the first place, and how, exactly, does exploration promote learning? One way to approach these process-oriented questions is with a developmental cascades perspective, which holds that new skills emerge from earlier-developing ones and that various interactions with people and objects accumulate over time to influence multiple domains of development (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). In this chapter, we describe object exploration from a developmental cascades perspective. In Section 2, we describe typical and atypical trajectories of exploration behaviors, noting how these behaviors emerge from earlier-developing cognitive and motor skills. In Section 3, we discuss how object exploration opens the door for new types of learning opportunities. In Section 4, we discuss early experiences that may shape the development of object exploration. Altogether, we aim to convey that new developments in exploration skills are extensions of earlier-developing skills, and that seemingly insignificant exploratory behaviors (e.g., shaking a rattle) may result in numerous and varied consequences for the developing infant.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    数字技术提供已被公认为一种社交和学习工具,幼儿园课程数字化的要求已经存在了几十年。孩子早年的数字化可能会与父母发生冲突,因为数字化社会对儿童数字技术使用的社会和经济需求与健康组织的指南和建议不一致,这些指南和建议警告学龄前儿童的技术使用。斯洛文尼亚的幼儿园课程改革是在1990年代进行的。在这个时期,使用数字学习技术和数字游戏对儿童学习倾向和早期识字能力的发展已经得到认可。在课程设计的时候,它集成了数字学习技术的一些元素。现在,新课程设计30年后,我们正面临着后数字时代。早期学习技术是与父母合作的问题;因此,这项描述性调查研究包括306名父母的非随机样本.我们正在考虑儿童游戏在初级环境中的结构,以及父母在当前的无缝后数字时代如何看待数字技术,数字化饱和的社会实践。父母意识到技术的风险及其对学习的好处。研究结果表明,孩子的数字屏幕技术使用与父母对数字游戏的态度和看法之间存在相关性。向孩子提供数字屏幕技术的父母对其对孩子的可能风险持不太满意的态度。
    Digital technology affordance has been recognized as a social and learning tool, and the requirements for digitalizing the kindergarten curriculum have been present for decades. Digitalization in a child\'s early years can present conflict with parents, as the societal and economic demands of digitalized society for a child\'s digital technology use are in dissonance with guidelines and recommendations of health organizations that caution against preschool children\'s technology use. Kindergarten curricular reform in Slovenia was conducted in the 1990s. In this period, the use of digital learning technology and digital play for the development of children learning predispositions and early literacy was already recognized. At the time of curriculum design, it integrated some elements of digital learning technology. Now, 30 years after the design of the new curriculum, we are facing the post-digital era. Learning technology in the early years is a matter of partnership with parents; accordingly, this descriptive survey study includes a non-randomized sample of 306 parents. We are considering how child\'s play is structured in the primary environment and how parents perceive digital technology in the current post-digital age of seamless, digitally saturated social practices. Parents are aware of the risks of technology and of its benefits for learning. The findings show correlations between a child\'s digital screen technology use and parents\' attitudes and perceptions of digital play. Parents that offer digital screen technology to a child have less agreeable attitudes regarding its possible risks to a child.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    从定性的方法和通过个人的语言和文化框架(通过对话共同构建知识的过程),这项工作探索了讲西班牙语的拉丁裔儿童母亲所重视的非正式背景因素,3至5岁,并在一家非营利性的加利福尼亚幼儿园注册。结果显示,拉丁裔母亲具有文化知识形式,这会影响孩子的教育经历,并参与直接和间接的算术环境。虽然个人数学经验和家庭实践不同,拉丁裔母亲根据社会文化方面体验和导航孩子的学习,影响他们如何支持孩子的数学学习。这项研究借鉴了社会文化学习理论,这些理论重视嵌入有意义的学习体验中的学习。家庭学习模式对教育实践有几点启示,特别是对于拉丁家庭,其父母的参与通常不像美国白人英语父母那样明显。
    Drawing from a qualitative approach and through individual pláticas (processes of co-constructing knowledge through dialogue) as a linguistic and cultural framework, this work explored informal contextual factors valued by Spanish-speaking Latina mothers of children, three to five years of age and enrolled in a nonprofit California preschool. Results revealed that Latina mothers have cultural forms of knowledge which impact their children\'s educational experiences and engage in direct and indirect numeracy environments. While personal math experiences and home practices differed, Latina mothers experience and navigate their children\'s learning based on sociocultural aspects, influencing how they support their children\'s math learning. This study drew from sociocultural learning theories that value learning embedded within meaningful learning experiences. Patterns of family learning have several implications for educational practice, especially for Latine families whose parent participation is typically not as visible as White American English-speaking parents.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    幼儿经常犯特殊的计数错误。当被要求计算多个项目集的单位时,比如派对上的家庭数量,他们经常计算离散项目(即,个人),而不是集合的数量(即,families).一种解释涉及儿童对构成单位的不完全理解,导致对离散项目的偏好。在这里,我们证明儿童对计数的不完全理解也起了作用。在4岁和5岁儿童的实验中(N=43),我们发现,即使孩子们能够命名集合,将项目分组到集合中,并与集合创建一对一的对应关系,然而,许多孩子无法将集合视为单位。我们得出的结论是,对计数抽象原理的新生理解也是某些儿童计数错误的原因。
    Young children frequently make a peculiar counting mistake. When asked to count units that are sets of multiple items, such as the number of families at a party, they often count discrete items (i.e., individual people) rather than the number of sets (i.e., families). One explanation concerns children\'s incomplete understanding of what constitutes a unit, resulting in a preference for discrete items. Here we demonstrate that children\'s incomplete understanding of counting also plays a role. In an experiment with 4- and 5-year-old children (N = 43), we found that even if children are able to name sets, group items into sets, and create one-to-one correspondences with sets, many children are nevertheless unable to count sets as units. We conclude that a nascent understanding of the abstraction principle of counting is also a cause of some children\'s counting errors.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:父亲通常被认为主要负责提供家庭的经济需求。然而,过去的研究表明,父亲参与育儿对孩子的整体成长和发展具有重要意义。很少有研究调查父亲在抚养幼儿中的角色,特别是反应灵敏的护理和刺激,在撒哈拉以南非洲。这里报道的这项研究是一项更大的研究的一部分,该研究旨在评估重要时刻(MTM)计划在改善赞比亚和肯尼亚农村社区幼儿的养育护理方面的有效性。MTM计划使用育儿赋权方法来促进照顾者与家庭内子女之间的联系和互动,专注于反应灵敏的护理,早期学习,以及安全和保障,以便儿童充分发挥其发展潜力。经过培训的志愿者促进了每月的主要照顾者支持和学习小组以及ECD家访。鼓励父亲参加家访,并参加有关特定主题的一些小组会议。本文报告的研究旨在确定育儿赋权计划在促进父亲(不是主要照顾者)对育儿的性别平等态度和做法方面的影响。
    方法:在三个时间点收集定性数据(实施开始前的干预前;实施6个月后的干预中;和干预后,24个月后)。我们与3岁以下儿童的主要照顾者(n=72)和父亲(n=24)进行了重点小组讨论。与ECD促进者(n=43)和信仰领袖(=20)进行了深入访谈。我们还对参与计划实施的MTM计划实施者(n=8)和政府官员(n=5)进行了关键的线人访谈。我们采用主题分析来分析定性数据。
    结果:研究结果表明,MTM计划改善了母亲/其他主要照顾者和父亲之间性别平等的育儿态度和做法。研究参与者报告说,由于参加MTM计划,大多数父亲花更多的时间与孩子玩耍和互动,并且更多地参与家务。
    结论:研究结果为政策制定提供了证据,并为政策的实施提供了指导,这些政策可以影响育儿中感知的性别角色的变化。
    BACKGROUND: Fathers are often perceived to be mainly responsible for the provision of the family\'s economic needs. However, past studies have demonstrated that fathers\' involvement in parenting has great significance for the child\'s holistic growth and development. Few studies have investigated fathers\' roles in the nurturing care of young children, particularly responsive care and stimulation, in sub-Saharan Africa. The study reported here was carried out as part of a larger study that sought to evaluate the effectiveness of the Moments That Matter (MTM) program in improving the nurturing care of young children in rural communities in Zambia and Kenya. The MTM program uses a parenting empowerment approach to promote bonding and interactions between caregivers and their children within the home, focusing on responsive care, early learning, and security and safety so that children reach their full developmental potential. Trained volunteers facilitated monthly primary caregiver support and learning groups and ECD home visits. Fathers were encouraged to participate in the home visits and to attend some of the group meetings on specific topics. The study reported in this paper aimed to establish the impact of the parenting empowerment program in promoting more gender-equal attitudes and practices on parenting among fathers (who were not the primary caregivers).
    METHODS: Qualitative data were collected at three time points (pre-intervention before the implementation began; mid-intervention after 6 months of implementation; and post-intervention, after 24 months). We conducted focused group discussions with primary caregivers (n = 72) and fathers (n = 24) with children below 3 years. In-depth interviews were conducted with ECD Promoters (n = 43) and faith leaders (= 20). We also conducted key informant interviews with the MTM program implementers (n = 8) and government officials (n = 5) involved in the program implementation. We employed thematic analysis to analyse the qualitative data.
    RESULTS: The findings showed that the MTM program resulted in improved gender-equal parenting attitudes and practices among mothers/other primary caregivers and fathers. Study participants reported that most fathers spent more time playing and interacting with their children and were more involved in household chores due to their participation in the MTM program.
    CONCLUSIONS: The study findings provide evidence for policy formulation and a guide for implementation of policies that can influence changes in perceived gender roles in parenting.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号