Conflict tasks

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    来自冲突任务的实验室数据,例如,西蒙和埃里克森任务,揭示了不同实验条件下响应时间分布的差异。直到最近,证据积累模型成功地再现了这些结果,特别是具有负斜率的具有挑战性的三角洲地块。它们在其结构或激活函数中具有明确的时间依赖性来实现这一点。在这项工作中,我们引入了一种替代方法来对冲突任务中的决策进行建模,该方法仅基于双路线体系结构中的抑制动力学。我们考虑同时自动和受控的漂移扩散过程,后者抑制前者。尽管没有时间相关的函数,但我们提出的双路证据累积模型(DREAM)在拟合实验响应时间分布方面实现了与以前的工作等效的性能。该模型可以再现具有正斜率和负斜率的条件精度函数和delta图。这些结果的含义,包括对参数的解释和与感知表示的潜在联系,正在讨论。我们提供Python代码来使DREAM适合实验数据。
    在线版本包含补充材料,可在10.1007/s11571-023-09990-8获得。
    Laboratory data from conflict tasks, e.g. Simon and Eriksen tasks, reveal differences in response time distributions under different experimental conditions. Only recently have evidence accumulation models successfully reproduced these results, in particular the challenging delta plots with negative slopes. They accomplish this with explicit temporal dependencies in their structure or activation functions. In this work, we introduce an alternative approach to the modeling of decision-making in conflict tasks exclusively based on inhibitory dynamics within a dual-route architecture. We consider simultaneous automatic and controlled drift diffusion processes, with the latter inhibiting the former. Our proposed Dual-Route Evidence Accumulation Model (DREAM) achieves equivalent performance to previous works in fitting experimental response time distributions despite having no time-dependent functions. The model can reproduce conditional accuracy functions and delta plots with positive and negative slopes. The implications of these results, including an interpretation of the parameters and potential links to perceptual representations, are discussed. We provide Python code to fit DREAM to experimental data.
    UNASSIGNED: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11571-023-09990-8.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在本研究中,我们使用块式(实验1)和试验式(实验2和3)操作来表示奖励的可能性,通过手动响应(实验1和2:Simon和Stroop任务;实验3:Simon和Eriksen侧翼任务),研究了绩效或有奖励前景对任务绩效的影响。在所有实验中,与每个冲突任务中的无奖励条件相比,任务性能(反应时间和/或错误率)通常在奖励方面有所改善。然而,有,如果有的话,很少有证据表明奖励操纵调节了平均冲突效应的大小,并且在通过分布分析(delta图)控制冲突处理中的时变波动时,也没有证据表明奖励的特定冲突效果。因此,结果没有为冲突特定账户提供证据,而是倾向于绩效一般账户,其中奖励预期导致整体性能改进而不影响冲突效应。我们讨论了主动控制如何调节冲突任务中目标处理和干扰处理之间的相互作用的可能含义。
    In the present study, we investigated the influence of performance-contingent reward prospects on task performance across three visual conflict tasks with manual responses (Experiments 1 & 2: Simon and Stroop tasks; Experiment 3: Simon and Eriksen flanker task) using block-wise (Experiment 1) and trial-wise (Experiments 2 & 3) manipulations to signal the possibility of reward. Across all experiments, task performance (in reaction time and/or error rates) generally improved in reward compared with no-reward conditions in each conflict task. However, there was, if any, little evidence that the reward manipulation modulated the size of the mean conflict effects, and there was also no evidence for conflict-specific effects of reward when controlling for time-varying fluctuations in conflict processing via distributional analyses (delta plots). Thus, the results provide no evidence for conflict-specific accounts and instead favor performance-general accounts, where reward anticipation leads to overall performance improvements without affecting conflict effects. We discuss possible implications for how proactive control might modulate the interplay between target- and distractor-processing in conflict tasks.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    本研究调查了全球行为适应对不同干扰方式引起的冲突的影响。使用具有恒定视觉目标的Eriksen侧翼范例进行了三个实验,而是随机变化的听觉或视觉干扰物。在实验1中,两种干扰方式的一致试验与不一致试验的比例都不同,而在实验2A和2B中,将这种比例一致性(PC)操作应用于使用一种干扰方式(诱导物)的试验,以测试使用另一种干扰方式(诊断)的试验的潜在行为转移效应.在所有的实验中,平均比例一致性效应(PCE)存在于使用PC操作的试验中,但是在实验2A和2B中没有转移到诊断试验的证据。分布分析(delta图)通过在增加全等试验的比率时显示出视觉而非听觉干扰物的delta图的斜率差异,为干扰物模态特定的全局行为适应提供了进一步的证据。因此,有人建议,由于学习特定于模态的记忆痕迹,干扰者模态限制了全局行为适应效应(例如,干扰者-目标关联)和/或特定于模态的认知控制过程(例如,抑制特定于模态的基于干扰物的激活)。此外,其他分析显示,在不同干扰模式的试验中,一致性序列效应部分转移,这表明干扰模式可能对局部和全局行为适应产生不同影响.
    The present study investigated global behavioral adaptation effects to conflict arising from different distractor modalities. Three experiments were conducted using an Eriksen flanker paradigm with constant visual targets, but randomly varying auditory or visual distractors. In Experiment 1, the proportion of congruent to incongruent trials was varied for both distractor modalities, whereas in Experiments 2A and 2B, this proportion congruency (PC) manipulation was applied to trials with one distractor modality (inducer) to test potential behavioral transfer effects to trials with the other distractor modality (diagnostic). In all experiments, mean proportion congruency effects (PCEs) were present in trials with a PC manipulation, but there was no evidence of transfer to diagnostic trials in Experiments 2A and 2B. Distributional analyses (delta plots) provided further evidence for distractor modality-specific global behavioral adaptations by showing differences in the slope of delta plots with visual but not auditory distractors when increasing the ratio of congruent trials. Thus, it is suggested that distractor modalities constrain global behavioral adaptation effects due to the learning of modality-specific memory traces (e.g., distractor-target associations) and/or the modality-specific cognitive control processes (e.g., suppression of modality-specific distractor-based activation). Moreover, additional analyses revealed partial transfer of the congruency sequence effect across trials with different distractor modalities suggesting that distractor modality may differentially affect local and global behavioral adaptations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    最近开发的冲突任务扩散模型(DMC)Ulrich等人。(认知心理学,78,148-174,2015)很好地说明了来自所有标准冲突任务的数据(例如,Stroop,西蒙,和侧翼任务)在一个共同的证据积累框架内。DMC处理动力学的一个中心特征是,有一个快速积累干扰证据的初始阶段,然后随着处理的继续,有选择地从决策机制中撤出。我们认为,这种假设可能会令人不安,因为它可以被视为暗示干扰因素信息在处理过程中的定性变化。这些变化表明,不仅仅是简单的抑制或抑制干扰信息,因为它们涉及分心者随着时间的推移处理“改变标志”产生的证据。在这篇文章中,我们(a)开发了一种修订的DMC(RDMC),其动力学严格在抑制/抑制范围内运行(即,证据强度可以单调变化,但不能改变符号);(b)证明RDMC可以预测文献中观察到的全部δ图(即,正向和负向);(c)表明该模型对Simon和侧翼数据提供了很好的拟合,这些数据用于在个人和小组水平上对原始DMC进行基准测试。我们的模型提供了对Simon和侧翼任务之间处理差异的新颖说明。具体来说,它们在一致试验中如何处理干扰信息方面有所不同,而不是不一致的试验:西蒙任务中的一致试验显示,注意力从干扰信息转移相对较慢(即,位置),同时在侧翼任务中发生完整而快速的注意力转移。我们的新模型强调了在冲突处理中考虑自上而下的目标和自下而上的刺激作用之间的动态相互作用的重要性。
    The recently developed diffusion model for conflict tasks (DMC) Ulrich et al. (Cognitive Psychology, 78, 148-174, 2015) provides a good account of data from all standard conflict tasks (e.g., Stroop, Simon, and flanker tasks) within a common evidence accumulation framework. A central feature of DMC\'s processing dynamics is that there is an initial phase of rapid accumulation of distractor evidence that is then selectively withdrawn from the decision mechanism as processing continues. We argue that this assumption is potentially troubling because it could be viewed as implying qualitative changes in the representation of distractor information over the time course of processing. These changes suggest more than simple inhibition or suppression of distractor information, as they involve evidence produced by distractor processing \"changing sign\" over time. In this article, we (a) develop a revised DMC (RDMC) whose dynamics operate strictly within the limits of inhibition/suppression (i.e., evidence strength can change monotonically, but cannot change sign); (b) demonstrate that RDMC can predict the full range of delta plots observed in the literature (i.e., both positive-going and negative-going); and (c) show that the model provides excellent fits to Simon and flanker data used to benchmark the original DMC at both the individual and group level. Our model provides a novel account of processing differences across Simon and flanker tasks. Specifically, that they differ in how distractor information is processed on congruent trials, rather than incongruent trials: congruent trials in the Simon task show relatively slow attention shifting away from distractor information (i.e., location) while complete and rapid attention shifting occurs in the flanker task. Our new model highlights the importance of considering dynamic interactions between top-down goals and bottom-up stimulus effects in conflict processing.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    在冲突任务中,比如西蒙,埃里克森侧翼,或Stroop任务,与一致试验相比,不一致后,一致效应通常会降低:一致序列效应(CSE)。有人建议,在经历了相对于一致试验的不一致之后,CSE可能反映了对任务相关信息的处理增加和/或对任务无关信息的抑制。在本研究中,我们通过在CSE的背景下将冲突任务的扩散模型(DMC)框架应用于侧翼和西蒙任务来为这一讨论做出贡献。我们认为,DMC独立地对任务相关和任务无关的信息进行建模,因此是解开其独特贡献的第一个好候选人。作为第一种方法,我们将DMC联合或单独拟合到先前一致或不一致的试验中,使用四个经验侧翼和两个西蒙数据集。对于侧翼任务,我们安装了经典的DMC版本。对于西蒙的任务,我们安装了一个广义的DMC版本,该版本允许与任务无关的信息在摆动回零时下冲。在考虑模型拟合之后,我们提出第二种方法,我们实施了一种认知控制机制来模拟增加对任务相关信息的处理或增加对任务无关信息的抑制的影响。两种方法都表明,抑制与任务无关的信息对于创建典型的CSE模式至关重要。增加对任务相关信息的处理,然而,很少能准确地描述CSE。
    In conflict tasks, such as the Simon, Eriksen flanker, or Stroop task, the congruency effect is often reduced after an incongruent compared to a congruent trial: the congruency sequence effect (CSE). It was suggested that the CSE may reflect increased processing of task-relevant information and/or suppression of task-irrelevant information after experiencing an incongruent relative to a congruent trial. In the present study, we contribute to this discussion by applying the Diffusion Model for Conflict tasks (DMC) framework in the context of CSEs to flanker and Simon tasks. We argue that DMC independently models the task-relevant and task-irrelevant information and thus is a first good candidate for disentangling their unique contributions. As a first approach, we fitted DMC conjointly or separately to previously congruent or incongruent trials, using four empirical flanker and two Simon data sets. For the flanker task, we fitted the classical DMC version. For the Simon task, we fitted a generalized DMC version which allows the task-irrelevant information to undershoot when swinging back to zero. After considering the model fits, we present a second approach, where we implemented a cognitive control mechanism to simulate the influence of increased processing of task-relevant information or increased suppression of task-irrelevant information. Both approaches demonstrate that the suppression of task-irrelevant information is essential to create the typical CSE pattern. Increased processing of task-relevant information, however, could rarely describe the CSE accurately.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    复杂的认知任务需要不同的处理阶段(即冲突监测、注意力资源分配和刺激分类)。这些子过程的平衡可能会影响双语者和单语者在冲突任务上的性能差异。当前的研究调查了在具有中等监控需求的冲突任务中,双语对这些子过程的影响。在侧翼任务中,对双语者和单语者的行为反应和诱发电位进行了25%的不一致试验。通过平均响应时间和响应时间分布的指数修正高斯分析来分析行为差异。对于诱发电位,该研究集中于N2(反映冲突监测)和P3反应(反映认知控制的注意力资源分配).与单语者相比,双语者的反应分布尾巴明显更长。双语者与单语言者相比,具有更明显的N2和更小的P3,独立于条件,建议两组子过程的不同平衡。这表明双语者更积极地参与监测进程,导致在刺激分类过程中分配更少的注意力资源。此外,P3振幅与双语者反应分布尾的长度呈负相关。这些结果与加强双语者的冲突监测相一致,这导致注意力资源对刺激分类的参与减少。这种加强的冲突监测可能导致偶尔极其缓慢的反应。因此,双语体验似乎会影响冲突任务期间认知控制过程的平衡,这可能只反映在少数回应中。
    Complex cognitive tasks require different stages of processing (i.e. conflict monitoring, attentional resource allocation and stimulus categorisation). Performance differences between bilinguals and monolinguals on conflict tasks can be affected by the balance of these sub-processes. The current study investigated the effect of bilingualism on these sub-processes during a conflict task with medium monitoring demand. Behavioural responses and evoked potentials from bilinguals and monolinguals were examined during a flanker task with 25% incongruent trials. Behavioural differences were analysed by means of averaged response times and exponentially modified Gaussian analyses of response time distributions. For evoked potentials, the study focussed on N2 (reflecting conflict monitoring) and P3 responses (reflecting allocation of attentional resources for cognitive control). Bilinguals had significantly longer response distribution tails compared to monolinguals. Bilinguals were shown to have a more pronounced N2 and smaller P3 compared to monolinguals, independent of condition, suggesting a different balance of sub-processes for the two groups. This suggests that bilinguals were engaged more strongly in monitoring processes, leading to the allocation of fewer attentional resources during stimulus categorisation. Additionally, the P3 amplitudes were negatively related with the length of response distribution tails for bilinguals. These results are consistent with enhanced conflict monitoring in bilinguals that led to reduced engagement of attentional resources for stimulus categorisation. This enhanced conflict monitoring could lead to occasional extremely slow responses. Thus, the bilingual experience appears to impact the balance of cognitive control processes during conflict tasks, which might only be reflected in a minority of responses.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    关于测量内隐态度的个体差异的问题,到目前为止,这是这次交流的焦点,应该与更一般的问题区分开来,即内隐态度是否在我们的脑海中存在和运作。理论家经常从对第一组问题的悲观结果转向对第二组问题的悲观结论。也就是说,他们有证据表明,诸如内隐联想测验(IAT)之类的间接措施令人失望,并将其用作个体差异措施(错误地)表明人们实际上并没有针对被污名化的群体的内隐态度。在这篇评论中,我详细剖析了这个错误,从认知科学中关于“冲突任务”(如Stroop任务)的平行辩论中汲取关键教训。我认为,证据总体上支持一个微妙的结论:间接措施,如IAT测量内隐态度的个体差异,但是他们-通过不同的证据-仍然支持内隐态度存在的观点。本文分为:心理学>理论与方法。
    Questions about measurement of individual differences in implicit attitudes, which have been the focus so far in this exchange, should be distinguished from more general questions about whether implicit attitudes exist and operate in our minds. Theorists frequently move too quickly from pessimistic results regarding the first set of questions to pessimistic conclusions about the second. That is, they take evidence that indirect measures such as the implicit association test (IAT) disappoint as individual difference measures and use it to (mistakenly) suggest that people do not in fact have implicit attitudes directed at stigmatized groups. In this commentary, I dissect this mistake in detail, drawing key lessons from a parallel debate that has unfolded in cognitive science about \"conflict tasks\" such as the Stroop task. I argue that the evidence overall supports a nuanced conclusion: Indirect measures such as the IAT measure individual differences in implicit attitudes poorly, but they-via distinct lines of evidence-still support the view that implicit attitudes exist. This article is categorized under: Psychology > Theory and Methods.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The cognitive processes underlying the ability of human performers to trade speed for accuracy is often conceptualized within evidence accumulation models, but it is not yet clear whether and how these models can account for decision-making in the presence of various sources of conflicting information. In the present study, we provide evidence that speed-accuracy tradeoffs (SATs) can have opposing effects on performance across two different conflict tasks. Specifically, in a single preregistered experiment, the mean reaction time (RT) congruency effect in the Simon task increased, whereas the mean RT congruency effect in the Eriksen task decreased, when the focus was put on response speed versus accuracy. Critically, distributional RT analyses revealed distinct delta plot patterns across tasks, thus indicating that the unfolding of distractor-based response activation in time is sufficient to explain the opposing pattern of congruency effects. In addition, a recent evidence accumulation model with the notion of time-varying conflicting information was successfully fitted to the experimental data. These fits revealed task-specific time-courses of distractor-based activation and suggested that time pressure substantially decreases decision boundaries in addition to reducing the duration of non-decision processes and the rate of evidence accumulation. Overall, the present results suggest that time pressure can have multiple effects in decision-making under conflict, but that strategic adjustments of decision boundaries in conjunction with different time-courses of distractor-based activation can produce counteracting effects on task performance with different types of distracting sources of information.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    This article reviews the historical usage of the concept of \'conflict\' in psychology and delineates the design and development of three basic conflict tasks (Stroop, Flanker, Stop Signal). Afterwards, important theoretical concepts to account for conflict processing are introduced. In the second part, the usage of these tasks in clinical psychology is considered. The article closes with some reflections regarding factors that may have been hitherto largely neglected in this respect.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    两个实验检查了埃里克森任务内部和整个任务的全球和局部行为适应效应,冲突是基于刺激字母的身份,还有西蒙的任务,冲突是基于刺激和反应的位置。两个任务的试验是随机混合的,在两项任务(实验1)或一项任务(实验2)中,全等试验的列表比例不同.在Simon任务中,列表范围的一致性比例(LWPC效应)的全球适应效应至少与Eriksen任务一样大。同样,在Simon任务中,先前试验一致性的局部适应效应(Gratton效应)至少与Eriksen任务一样大.与之前调查Stroop和Simon任务转移的研究相反,全球和地方适应效应在不同冲突任务之间的转移没有分离。事实上,局部和全球适应效应在很大程度上出现了特定于任务的效应,因为没有或只有很少的Gratton效应或LWPC效应从Eriksen转移到Simon任务,反之亦然。总的来说,结果表明,在目前的设计中观察到的行为适应不会从这些任务中的一个延续到另一个,暗示没有更高阶的参与,任务-认知控制的一般机制。
    Two experiments examined global and local behavioral adaptation effects within and across the Eriksen task, where conflict is based on stimulus letter identities, and the Simon task, where conflict is based on stimulus and response locations. Trials of the two tasks were randomly intermixed, and the list-wide proportion of congruent trials was varied in both tasks (Experiment 1) or in just one task (Experiment 2). The global adaptation effect of list-wide congruency proportion (LWPC effect) was at least as large in the Simon task as in the Eriksen task. Likewise, the local adaptation effect of previous-trial congruency (Gratton effect) was at least as large in the Simon task as in the Eriksen task. In contrast to prior studies investigating transfer across Stroop and Simon tasks, there was no dissociation between global and local adaptation effects regarding their transfer across the different conflict tasks. In fact, both local and global adaptation effects appeared largely task-specific, because there was no or only little transfer of either Gratton effects or LWPC effects from the Eriksen to the Simon task or vice versa. On the whole, the results suggest that behavioral adaptation observed in the present design does not carry over from one of these tasks to the other, suggesting no involvement of a higher-order, task-general mechanism of cognitive control.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号