Canine Cognition

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    催产素和皮质醇是可以影响认知和行为的激素,但是内源性浓度与认知和行为表型的个体差异之间的关系仍然知之甚少。在哺乳动物中,催产素在多种社会行为中具有重要作用,在狗中,它与以人为本的行为有关,例如社会凝视和跟踪点。皮质醇,下丘脑-垂体-肾上腺(HPA)轴的最终产品,经常被研究与气质和情绪反应有关的问题,但它也可以调节执行功能。在这项研究中,我们测量了纯种人群(犬科动物®)的小狗的基础粪便皮质醇(n=247)和血浆催产素(n=249)。我们收集了这些受试者的认知和行为数据(n=247),包括以人为本的社会认知措施,记忆,抑制控制,知觉辨别,和气质。催产素浓度估计为高度可遗传(h2=0.90-0.99),皮质醇浓度估计为中度可遗传(h2=0.43-0.47)。控制相关性的贝叶斯混合模型显示,催产素浓度与空间工作记忆呈正相关,与行为偏侧呈负二次关系,但是没有可靠的社会措施协会。皮质醇浓度与抑制控制任务的性能呈负线性关系,与对新对象的大胆行为反应呈负二次关系。总的来说,我们的结果表明,在狗中,催产素和皮质醇浓度的个体差异受到强烈的遗传控制,并且与气质方面的表型变异有关。行为偏侧性,和执行功能。
    Oxytocin and cortisol are hormones that can influence cognition and behavior, but the relationships between endogenous concentrations and individual differences in cognitive and behavioral phenotypes remain poorly understood. Across mammals, oxytocin has important roles in diverse social behaviors, and in dogs, it has been implicated in human-oriented behaviors such as social gaze and point-following. Cortisol, an end-product of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, is often studied in relation to temperament and emotional reactivity, but it is also known to modulate executive functions. In this study, we measured basal fecal cortisol (n = 247) and plasma oxytocin (n = 249) in dog puppies from a pedigreed population (Canine Companions ®). We collected cognitive and behavioral data from these subjects (n = 247), including measures of human-oriented social cognition, memory, inhibitory control, perceptual discriminations, and temperament. Oxytocin concentrations were estimated to be very highly heritable (h2 = 0.90-0.99) and cortisol concentrations were estimated to be moderately-highly heritable (h2 = 0.43-0.47). Bayesian mixed models controlling for relatedness revealed that oxytocin concentrations were positively associated with spatial working memory and displayed a negative quadratic relationship with behavioral laterality, but no credible associations were seen for social measures. Cortisol concentrations exhibited a negative linear relationship with performance on an inhibitory control task and a negative quadratic relationship with bold behavioral reactions to a novel object. Collectively, our results suggest that individual differences in oxytocin and cortisol concentrations are under strong genetic control in dogs and are associated with phenotypic variation in aspects of temperament, behavioral laterality, and executive function.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    基于机器学习的行为跟踪是行为科学中一种快速增长的方法,可以提供具有高空间和时间分辨率的数据,同时降低观察者偏差的风险。然而,只有少数犬行为研究应用了这种方法。在目前的研究中,我们使用三维(3D)跟踪狗的身体来研究与看护者的分离如何影响狗在新环境中的行为。在研究期间,狗可以在配备有试验独特物体的房间里自由活动。我们操纵了整个试验,无论主人和/或陌生人是否在场,以评估安全的基础效果,与另一个人相比,在照顾者面前探索和玩耍的倾向更多。这种安全的基础效应被认为是人类依恋纽带的关键特征,也已针对狗与看护者的关系进行了描述。跟踪数据在内部是一致的,并且与人类评分和测量结果高度相关。结果表明,主人和陌生人都显着增加了狗的探索;当他们在场时,狗也花了更多的时间在主人和陌生人位置附近。即使主人和陌生人的存在对狗的行为产生了重大影响,业主的影响更加明显。此外,在陌生人在场的情况下,狗花费更多的时间靠近它们的主人,并显示出减少的尾巴摇摆的不对称,以右侧进一步支持主人和陌生人对狗的行为的明显影响。我们得出的结论是,机器驱动的3D跟踪为狗的探索和依恋相关行为的详细行为分析提供了有效和可靠的访问。
    Machine-learning-based behavioural tracking is a rapidly growing method in the behavioural sciences providing data with high spatial and temporal resolution while reducing the risk of observer bias. Nevertheless, only a few canine behaviour studies have applied this method. In the current study, we used three-dimensional (3D) tracking of the dogs\' bodies to study how separation from the caregiver affected the dogs\' behaviour in a novel environment. During the study, the dogs could move freely in a room equipped with trial-unique objects. We manipulated across trials whether the owner and/or a stranger was present in the room to evaluate the secure base effect, the tendency to explore and play more in the presence of the caregiver compared to another person. This secure base effect is considered a key characteristic of human attachment bonds and has also been described for the dog-caregiver relationship. The tracking data were internally consistent and highly correlated with human scorings and measurements. The results show that both the owner and stranger significantly increased the dogs\' exploration; the dogs also spent more time in the proximity of the owner and stranger location when they were present. Even though the presence of both owner and stranger had a significant effect on the dogs\' behaviour, the effect of the owner was more pronounced. Moreover, in the presence of the stranger the dogs spent more time close to their owner and showed a reduced tail-wagging asymmetry to the right side further supporting the distinct effect of owner and stranger on the dogs\' behaviour. We conclude that machine-driven 3D tracking provides an efficient and reliable access for detailed behavioural analyses of dogs\' exploration and attachment-related behaviours.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    多年来,公民科学方法越来越受欢迎,部分原因是它们能够接触到更广泛的受众,并产生更普遍的样本。在狗中,这些研究,虽然,对材料或实验方案的控制受到限制,监护人通常在没有研究人员监督的情况下报告结果。在两项研究中,我们探索并验证了一种同步公民科学方法。我们让狗监护人充当实验者,同时由研究人员监督Zoom。在研究1中,我们证明了同步公民科学在选择任务中产生了与实验室设计相同的性能水平。与过去的实验室研究一致,狗选择了一种治疗(与空盘子)在两个替代强制选择任务中。在研究2中,我们表明Zoom方法也适用于利用观察时间度量的研究。我们探索了当一袋零食被放置在一个无法到达的地方时,狗的外观行为,当狗试图取回零食时,狗的监护人要么专心,要么不专心。与过去的工作一致,处于专注状态的狗比处于不专注状态的狗看监护人的时间更长,并且第一次看的潜伏期更短。总的来说,我们已经证明,与狗进行同步公民科学研究是可行的,并且产生的结果与典型实验室环境中的结果一致。
    Citizen science approaches have grown in popularity over the years, partly due to their ability to reach a wider audience and produce more generalizable samples. In dogs, these studies, though, have been limited in their controls over materials or experimental protocols, with guardians typically reporting results without researcher supervision. Over two studies, we explored and validated a synchronous citizen science approach. We had dog guardians act as experimenters while being supervised by a researcher over Zoom. In study 1, we demonstrated that synchronous citizen science produced equivalent levels of performance to in-lab designs in a choice task. Consistent with past in-lab research, dogs selected a treat (vs. an empty plate) in a two-alternative forced-choice task. In study 2, we showed that Zoom methods are also appropriate for studies utilizing looking time measures. We explored dogs\' looking behaviors when a bag of treats was placed in an unreachable location, and dogs\' guardians were either attentive or inattentive while dogs attempted to retrieve the treats. Consistent with past work, dogs in the attentive condition looked at their guardian for longer periods and had a shorter latency to first look than dogs in the inattentive condition. Overall, we have demonstrated that synchronous citizen science studies with dogs are feasible and produce valid results consistent with those found in a typical lab setting.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    零售狗玩具通常提供给伴侣动物,以提供认知和身体刺激,并改善动物的生活质量。这些玩具,有时被称为“浓缩玩具”,已被证明在增加食欲和活动水平以及减少不良行为方面发挥作用(例如,吠叫,自我隔离行为)在某些家犬(犬类)人群中。在这项研究中,我们评估玩具对食欲的影响,活动水平,和积极的情感状态作为伴侣犬的幸福感的衡量标准。比较了在十二天内与不同类型的玩具进行定期互动之前和之后的行为。我们发现,为伴侣犬提供玩具并没有显着改变它们的活动水平,食物消费率,或者认知偏见。虽然接受更复杂玩具的狗表现出稍微改善的认知偏见,接受“不太复杂”玩具的受试者之间的行为没有显著差异(例如,一根骨头,球)和“更复杂的”玩具(例如,益智玩具)。最后,我们反思了我们的主题人群与所看到的结果的相关性,以及认知偏差测试的不同形式。
    Retail dog toys are often provided to companion animals to provide cognitive and physical stimu- lation and improve the animals\' quality of life. These toys, sometimes known as \"enrichment toys\", have been shown to play a role in increasing appetite and activity levels and decreasing undesirable behaviors (e.g., barking, self-isolating behaviors) in some domestic dog (Canis familiaris) populations. In this study, we evaluate the effect of toys on appetite, activity levels, and positive affective states as measures of well- being in companion dogs. Behaviors were compared before and after regular interactions with different types of toys over twelve days. We found that provisioning companion dogs with toys did not significantly alter their activity level, rate of food consumption, or cognitive bias. While dogs who received more complex toys showed a slightly improved cognitive bias, there were no significant differences in behaviors be- tween the subjects who received \"less complex\" toys (e.g., a bone, ball) and \"more complex\" toys (e.g., puzzle toys). We conclude with reflections on the relevance of our subject population to the result seen, and on the different forms of the cognitive bias test.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    预测他人行为的能力是社会认知的主要支柱之一。我们通过将观察到的运动的运动表示与视觉熟悉度进行对比,研究了这种能力的基础过程。在两个预先登记的眼动追踪实验中,我们测量了16只狗(Canisfamiliaris)的视线到达时间,这些狗观察了人类或特定动物执行相同目标定向动作的视频。在第一次审判中,当人类代理人在狗之外进行人类典型的运动时,狗的目光正向地到达目标物体(即,在人类触摸目标物体之前)。当特工是特定的时候,狗的目光反应性地到达目标物体(即,触摸时或触摸后)。当人类代理人进行与狗运动可能性更密切相关的不寻常运动时(例如,爬行而不是走路),狗的凝视到达时间介于其他两个条件之间。在复制实验中,有稍微不同的刺激,狗对目标物体的观察既没有显著的预测性也没有反应性,不管是代理人。然而,当包括对目标对象的查看时,前面没有对代理的查看,平均而言,狗在人类代理人的行动目标上比在特定的目标上看起来更快。观察时间和瞳孔大小分析表明,狗的注意力更多地被狗代理人所吸引。这些结果表明,在预期动作目标时,对观察到的动作和显着性的视觉熟悉程度对狗的观察行为的影响要比特定于效应器的运动表示更强。
    The ability to predict others\' actions is one of the main pillars of social cognition. We investigated the processes underlying this ability by pitting motor representations of the observed movements against visual familiarity. In two pre-registered eye-tracking experiments, we measured the gaze arrival times of 16 dogs (Canis familiaris) who observed videos of a human or a conspecific executing the same goal-directed actions. On the first trial, when the human agent performed human-typical movements outside dogs\' specific motor repertoire, dogs\' gaze arrived at the target object anticipatorily (i.e., before the human touched the target object). When the agent was a conspecific, dogs\' gaze arrived to the target object reactively (i.e., upon or after touch). When the human agent performed unusual movements more closely related to the dogs\' motor possibilities (e.g., crawling instead of walking), dogs\' gaze arrival times were intermediate between the other two conditions. In a replication experiment, with slightly different stimuli, dogs\' looks to the target object were neither significantly predictive nor reactive, irrespective of the agent. However, when including looks at the target object that were not preceded by looks to the agents, on average dogs looked anticipatorily and sooner at the human agent\'s action target than at the conspecific\'s. Looking times and pupil size analyses suggest that the dogs\' attention was captured more by the dog agent. These results suggest that visual familiarity with the observed action and saliency of the agent had a stronger influence on the dogs\' looking behaviour than effector-specific movement representations in anticipating action targets.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    先前对人类婴儿的研究表明,违反基本的身体规律可以刺激探索,这可能代表了一种旨在获取有关新因果关系的知识的假设检验。在这项研究中,我们研究了在非人类动物中是否存在违反预期和探索之间的类似联系。具体来说,我们调查了狗在发生闭塞事件时对违反预期行为的反应.在三个实验中,与预期不一致的事件相比,狗的观察时间更长。在前两个眼睛跟踪实验中,瞳孔大小分析进一步支持了这一发现。我们的结果表明,狗希望对象在不被屏幕遮挡时重新出现,并考虑遮挡屏幕相对于被遮挡对象的大小。在实验3中,违反预期的行为增加了狗对目标物体的探索,与人类婴儿的发现相似。我们得出的结论是,违反预期行为也可以为非人类动物提供学习机会。
    Previous research on human infants has shown that violations of basic physical regularities can stimulate exploration, which may represent a type of hypothesis testing aimed at acquiring knowledge about new causal relationships. In this study, we examined whether a similar connection between expectancy violation and exploration exists in nonhuman animals. Specifically, we investigated how dogs react to expectancy violations in the context of occlusion events. Throughout three experiments, dogs exhibited longer looking times at expectancy-inconsistent events than at consistent ones. This finding was further supported by pupil size analyses in the first two eye-tracking experiments. Our results suggest that dogs expect objects to reappear when they are not obstructed by a screen and consider the size of the occluding screen in relation to the occluded object. In Experiment 3, expectancy violations increased the dogs\' exploration of the target object, similar to the findings with human infants. We conclude that expectancy violations can provide learning opportunities for nonhuman animals as well.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    联合意向,对共同目标或参与共同任务的行动的相互理解,被认为是人类心理理论的重要组成部分。驯化的狗异常善于理解人类的社会线索并与人类合作,使它们在与人类的互动中拥有共同意图的行为特征成为可能。Horschler及其同事(AnimBehav183:159-168,2022)检查了服务犬种群的联合意图,发现在共同经历中断后,狗优先与前伴侣重新接触,而不是被动的旁观者,一种行为被认为是人类儿童共同意图的标志。在目前的研究中,我们的目标是在宠物狗中复制和扩展这些结果。一个熟悉的人和狗玩耍,然后突然停了下来。我们检查了狗是否会优先重新吸引玩家,而不是也在场的熟悉的旁观者。与Horschler及其同事的发现一致(AnimBehav183:159-168,2022),与熟悉的旁观者相比,宠物狗优先凝视并向玩家提供玩具。然而,在身体接触方面没有观察到差异。这些发现为宠物狗的联合意图行为特征提供了初步证据,但是需要未来的工作来了解这种现象是否扩展到其他环境。
    Joint intentionality, the mutual understanding of shared goals or actions to partake in a common task, is considered an essential building block of theory of mind in humans. Domesticated dogs are unusually adept at comprehending human social cues and cooperating with humans, making it possible that they possess behavioral signatures of joint intentionality in interactions with humans. Horschler and colleagues (Anim Behav 183: 159-168, 2022) examined joint intentionality in a service dog population, finding that upon interruption of a joint experience, dogs preferentially re-engaged their former partner over a passive bystander, a behavior argued to be a signature of joint intentionality in human children. In the current study, we aimed to replicate and extend these results in pet dogs. One familiar person played with the dog and then abruptly stopped. We examined if dogs would preferentially re-engage the player instead of a familiar bystander who was also present. Consistent with the findings of Horschler and colleagues (Anim Behav 183: 159-168, 2022), pet dogs preferentially gazed toward and offered the toy to the player significantly more than the familiar bystander. However, no difference was observed in physical contact. These findings provide preliminary evidence for behavioral signatures of joint intentionality in pet dogs, but future work is needed to understand whether this phenomenon extends to other contexts.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    作为驯化物种的狗(犬科动物)在多大程度上理解人类的意图仍然是一个争论的问题。已经开发了不愿意-无法实现的范式,以检查非人类动物是否对人类行为背后的意图敏感。在这个范式中,与不愿意(戏弄)的人相比,当出现一个看起来愿意但无法将食物转移给他们的人时,受试者倾向于在测试区域等待更长的时间。在本研究中,我们使用基于机器学习驱动的三维跟踪的详细行为分析,对狗进行了“不愿意-不能”模式.在两次预先登记的实验中,我们找到了证据,符合我们的预测,狗对表示不愿意转移食物而不是没有能力的行为更加不耐烦。这些差异在两个不同的宠物狗样本(总共n=96)中是一致的,并且它们在机器学习生成的三维跟踪数据中也是明显的。因此,我们的结果提供了有力的证据,证明狗可以区分与不同意图相关的相似动作(导致相同结果)。然而,在随后的转移阶段,他们的反应没有导致一个实验者对另一个实验者的任何可测量的偏好。我们讨论了在这种范式中可能构成狗表现的不同认知机制。
    The extent to which dogs (Canis familiaris) as a domesticated species understand human intentions is still a matter of debate. The unwilling-unable paradigm has been developed to examine whether nonhuman animals are sensitive to intentions underlying human actions. In this paradigm, subjects tended to wait longer in the testing area when presented with a human that appeared willing but unable to transfer food to them compared to an unwilling (teasing) human. In the present study, we conducted the unwilling-unable paradigm with dogs using a detailed behavioural analysis based on machine-learning driven three-dimensional tracking. Throughout two preregistered experiments, we found evidence, in line with our prediction, that dogs reacted more impatiently to actions signalling unwillingness to transfer food rather than inability. These differences were consistent through two different samples of pet dogs (total n = 96) and they were evident also in the machine-learning generated three-dimensional tracking data. Our results therefore provide robust evidence that dogs distinguish between similar actions (leading to the same outcome) associated with different intentions. However, their reactions did not lead to any measurable preference for one experimenter over the other in a subsequent transfer phase. We discuss different cognitive mechanisms that might underlie dogs\' performance in this paradigm.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    UNASSIGNED:这项研究的目的是评估衰老犬的参与认知挑战性和潜在的令人沮丧的任务(不可能的任务)。根据以前的观察,我们预测,在其他认知测试和主人填写的问卷中显示认知障碍迹象的狗将显示对任务的参与减少。
    UNASSIGNED:在此任务中,狗被展示在一个透明的容器中的一块食物,它们无法打开;测量与容器和实验者互动的时间。虽然不可能完成的任务还没有被用来衡量挫折,测试设计的参数会产生潜在的挫败状态,使这一评估适当。参加纵向衰老研究的32只狗参与了该研究。要求所有者完成两份认知功能障碍筛查问卷(犬痴呆量表[CADES]和犬认知功能障碍评定量表[CCDR])以及评估总体挫败感的问卷。狗参与了认知功能的多种测量以及不可能完成的任务。
    UNASSIGNED:对于总(受损)CADES(p=0.02)和总CCDR(p=0.04)评分较高的狗,脱离不可能完成的任务的延迟更快。在观察社会线索的认知测试中,脱离接触的延迟也与表现下降相关(p=0.01),工作记忆(p≤0.001),空间推理和反转学习(p=0.02),持续关注(p=0.02)。
    UNASSIGNED:与几种认知测量的高度相关性以及该测试的易于管理使其成为评估犬认知功能障碍综合征的有用工具,然而,目前尚不清楚挫折感或其他认知过程的增加是否有助于观察到的变化.
    UNASSIGNED: The aim of this study was to evaluate the engagement of aging dogs with a cognitively challenging and potentially frustrating task (the impossible task). Based on previous observations, we predicted that dogs showing signs of cognitive impairment in other cognitive tests and owner-completed questionnaires would show reduced engagement with the task.
    UNASSIGNED: In this task, dogs were shown a piece of food in a clear container that they could not open; time spent interacting with the container and the experimenter was measured. While the impossible task has not been used as a measure of frustration, the parameters of the test design creates a potential frustrate state, making this assessment appropriate. Thirty-two dogs enrolled in a longitudinal aging study participated in the study. Owners were asked to complete two cognitive dysfunction screening questionnaires (Canine Dementia Scale [CADES] and Canine Cognitive Dysfunction Rating Scale [CCDR]) as well a questionnaire assessing general frustration. Dogs participated in multiple measures of cognitive function as well the impossible task.
    UNASSIGNED: Latency to disengage from the impossible task was faster for dogs with higher total (more impaired) CADES (p = 0.02) and total CCDR (p = 0.04) scores. Latency to disengage also correlated with decreased performance in cognitive tests observing social cues (p = 0.01), working memory (p ≤ 0.001), spatial reasoning and reversal learning (p = 0.02), and sustained attention (p = 0.02).
    UNASSIGNED: The high correlation with several cognitive measures and the ease of administration of this test makes it a useful tool in evaluating canine cognitive dysfunction syndrome, however it is unclear if increased frustration or other cognitive processes are contributing to the observed changes.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    接触因果关系是使我们能够理解我们的物理环境的基本原则之一。从很小的时候,人类认为时空连续的发射事件是因果关系。令人惊讶的是,对非人类动物的因果感知知之甚少,特别是在灵长类秩序之外。与眼睛跟踪和瞳孔测量相结合的违反期望范例已用于研究人类婴儿的身体期望。在目前的研究中,我们为狗建立了这种方法(犬)。我们为狗提供了逼真的三维动画,这些动画显示了与所涉及对象之间的接触(定期发射事件)或不接触的发射事件。在这两种情况下,以相同的时序和运动学属性移动的对象。在整个研究过程中,狗密切跟踪了物体的运动,但在无接触条件下,它们的瞳孔更大,并且与接触事件相比,在无接触事件后,它们对启动发射的物体的观察时间更长。我们得出的结论是,狗对接触因果关系有隐含的期望。
    Contact causality is one of the fundamental principles allowing us to make sense of our physical environment. From an early age, humans perceive spatio-temporally contiguous launching events as causal. Surprisingly little is known about causal perception in non-human animals, particularly outside the primate order. Violation-of-expectation paradigms in combination with eye-tracking and pupillometry have been used to study physical expectations in human infants. In the current study, we establish this approach for dogs (Canis familiaris). We presented dogs with realistic three-dimensional animations of launching events with contact (regular launching event) or without contact between the involved objects. In both conditions, the objects moved with the same timing and kinematic properties. The dogs tracked the object movements closely throughout the study but their pupils were larger in the no-contact condition and they looked longer at the object initiating the launch after the no-contact event compared to the contact event. We conclude that dogs have implicit expectations about contact causality.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号