Blur

模糊
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    司机必须迅速应对各种可能的道路危险,从驶入车道的卡车到踏上道路的行人。当驾驶员的视力在执照点进行测试时,视觉功能会退化,驾驶员可能没有注意到这些变化如何影响他们及时注意和响应世界事件的能力。为了安全地检查视觉退化对危险检测的潜在后果,我们进行了两个实验,检查模拟光学模糊对参与者在危险检测任务中查看持续时间阈值的影响,作为方向盘后面的眼睛在路上的持续时间的代理。与年长和年轻的参与者一起检查这个问题,在两个实验中,我们发现在模糊条件下观看持续时间阈值总体增加,这样,年轻人和老年人同样受到模糊的影响。严重的,在这两组中,我们发现,对于非车辆道路危险(行人,骑自行车的人和动物)与车辆道路危险(汽车,卡车和公共汽车)。这项工作表明,模糊对检测非车辆道路使用者的驾驶员来说是一个特殊的问题,在碰撞中,人口比车辆道路使用者更容易受到伤害。这些结果还强调了在考虑模糊对道路危险检测的影响时考虑危险类型的重要性。
    Drivers must respond promptly to a wide range of possible road hazards, from trucks veering into their lane to pedestrians stepping onto the road. While drivers\' vision is tested at the point of licensure, visual function can degrade, and drivers may not notice how these changes impact their ability to notice and respond to events in the world in a timely fashion. To safely examine the potential consequences of visual degradation on hazard detection, we performed two experiments examining the impact of simulated optical blur on participants\' viewing duration thresholds in a hazard detection task, as a proxy for eyes-on-road duration behind the wheel. Examining this question with older and younger participants, across two experiments, we found an overall increase in viewing duration thresholds under blurred conditions, such that younger and older adults were similarly impacted by blur. Critically, in both groups, we found that the increment in thresholds produced by blur was larger for non-vehicular road hazards (pedestrians, cyclists and animals) compared to vehicular road hazards (cars, trucks and buses). This work suggests that blur poses a particular problem for drivers detecting non-vehicular road users, a population considerably more vulnerable in a collision than vehicular road users. These results also highlight the importance of taking into account the type of hazard when considering the impacts of blur on road hazard detection.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    怀孕会导致所有身体系统的变化,包括眼睛。眼睛可以在怀孕期间经历生理和病理变化。一些变化加剧了先前存在的眼部疾病,而其他疾病在怀孕期间首次出现。早期识别和管理对于预防视力威胁并发症至关重要。此外,一些产科并发症可能与眼科征象有关.迅速识别这些眼睛发现可能会挽救母亲和胎儿的生命。本文的目的是介绍妊娠中潜在的眼部并发症,并概述适当的管理方法以保护视力并保持母婴安全。还将讨论使用常见眼科药物的安全性。
    Pregnancy causes changes in all body systems, including the eye. The eye can undergo physiological and pathological changes in pregnancy. Some changes exacerbate pre-existing eye conditions while other conditions manifest for the first-time during pregnancy. Early recognition and management are essential to prevent sight threatening complications. In addition, some obstetric complications can be associated with ophthalmic signs. Prompt recognition of these eye findings may be life saving for both the mother and the foetus. The aim of this article is to present potential ocular complications in pregnancy and outline the appropriate management to preserve sight and maintain maternal and foetal safety. The safety of the use of common ophthalmological medications will also be discussed.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    FIB-SEM层析成像是一种连续切片技术,其中聚焦离子束(FIB)从正在分析的材料样品中切掉切片。每次切片后,所拍摄的扫描电子显微镜(SEM)图像显示了样品的新曝光层。通过组合堆栈中的所有切片,生成材料的3D图像。然而,由成像技术引起的特定伪影使图像失真,阻碍了结构的形态分析。显微镜成像中的典型质量问题是噪声和缺乏对比度或焦点。此外,特定的伪影是由FIB铣削引起的,即,遮蔽和充电文物。我们提出了用于评估FIB-SEM数据集质量的质量指标。在不同结构和材料的实际和实验数据上验证了指标。本文受版权保护。保留所有权利。
    Focused ion beam scanning electron microscopy (FIB-SEM) tomography is a serial sectioning technique where an FIB mills off slices from the material sample that is being analysed. After every slicing, an SEM image is taken showing the newly exposed layer of the sample. By combining all slices in a stack, a 3D image of the material is generated. However, specific artefacts caused by the imaging technique distort the images, hampering the morphological analysis of the structure. Typical quality problems in microscopy imaging are noise and lack of contrast or focus. Moreover, specific artefacts are caused by the FIB milling, namely, curtaining and charging artefacts. We propose quality indices for the evaluation of the quality of FIB-SEM data sets. The indices are validated on real and experimental data of different structures and materials.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    立体视取决于最小立体阈值(下限)和融合上限。虽然研究表明,下限随着对比度和模糊的降低而恶化,单眼比双眼疾病更强,对上限的影响仍然不确定。这里,我们评估了立体字母识别任务中对比度和模糊对视差灵敏度函数(DSF)范围的影响.受试者必须识别嵌入随机点立体图中的立体字母,和自适应楼梯被用来估计这两个极限。五名受试者在基线对比(100%)进行了实验,在单眼和双眼退化中具有不同的对比度(32%和10%)和模糊(0.75DS和1.25DS)。我们提出了三种可能的结果:1)范围在两个方向上崩溃2)下限阈值降低,但上限不受影响3)两个限制的阈值增加,范围保持不变。我们发现两个极限的曲线形状都很低,在较高的SF下导致较小的范围。结果与第一个预测相似,在较低的对比度和较高的模糊下,下限的阈值增加,而上限降低。DSF的收缩在单眼病症中是显著的。然而,与模糊,有受试者间的变异性。使用简单的互相关立体匹配算法来量化对比度和模糊的影响。结果与DSF范围随图像退化而减小的行为结果一致。
    Stereopsis depends on the smallest stereo threshold (lower limit) and the upper fusion limit. While studies have shown that the lower limit worsens with reduced contrast and blur, more strongly in monocular than in binocular conditions, the effect on the upper limit remains uncertain. Here, we assess the impact of contrast and blur on the range of the disparity sensitivity function (DSF) in a stereo letter recognition task. Subjects had to identify the stereo letters embedded in a random dot stereogram, and adaptive staircases were used to estimate the two limits. Five subjects performed the experiment at baseline contrast (100%), with different contrast (32% and 10%) and blur (+0.75DS and +1.25DS) in monocular and binocular degradation. We proposed three possible outcomes: 1) the range collapses in both directions 2) the lower limit threshold reduces, but the upper limit is not affected 3) the threshold for both limits increases and the range remains the same. We found that the curve for both limits was lowpass in shape, resulting in a smaller range at higher SFs. The results were similar to the first prediction, where the threshold for the lower limit increased while the upper limit was reduced at lower contrast and higher blur. The shrinkage of DSF is significant in monocular conditions. However, with blur, there was inter-subject variability. A simple cross-correlation stereo-matching algorithm was used to quantify the effect of contrast and blur. The results were consistent with the behavioral result that the range of DSF decreases with image degradation.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    背景:白内障在吸收方面会影响眼睛的光学,blur,和散射。当白内障是单方面的,它们引起眼睛之间的差异,会产生视觉不适并损害双眼视觉。这些眼间差异也会引起眼睛处理速度的差异,从而可能引起自发的Pulfrich效应,引起重要深度误解的视觉错觉。光级的两眼间差异,像那些出现在单侧白内障中的人,会引起经典的Pulfrich效应,和模糊的眼间差异,就像monovision中的那些人一样,老花眼的常见矫正方法,会导致反向Pulfrich效应。视觉系统可能能够适应,或者不是,在新的光学条件下,取决于白内障的程度和单视矫正的程度。
    方法:这里,我们报告了一例45岁患者的独特病例,该患者接受了单侧白内障手术,导致2.5屈光度(D)的单视矫正:手术后的左眼正视眼补偿了单焦点人工晶状体和右眼近视,球面等效为-2.50D。这可以用自发的Pulfrich效应(测量的延迟为4.82ms,这可以用0.19的光密度滤波器消除)。在第二只眼睛中取出清晰的晶状体摘除单视后,症状消失。我们证明,至少在这个病人身上,单侧白内障手术后,经典和反向Pulfrich效应并存,可以通过恢复眼间差异来重新适应。此外,我们报告说,适应/重新适应过程的反向Pulfrich效应发生在几周的时间范围内,与经典的Pulfrich效应相反,已知有几天的时间范围。此外,我们使用实验室测量的错觉来量化自发Pulfrich效应在不同视觉场景和任务中的相关性,使用几何模型和光流算法。
    结论:测量Pulfrich效应的不同版本可能有助于了解白内障手术后或单视手术患者报告的视觉不适,并可以指导补偿或干预策略。
    BACKGROUND: Cataracts affect the optics of the eye in terms of absorption, blur, and scattering. When cataracts are unilateral, they cause differences between the eyes that can produce visual discomfort and harm binocular vision. These interocular differences can also induce differences in the processing speed of the eyes that may cause a spontaneous Pulfrich effect, a visual illusion provoking important depth misperceptions. Interocular differences in light level, like those present in unilateral cataracts, can cause the Classic Pulfrich effect, and interocular differences in blur, like those present in monovision, a common correction for presbyopia, can cause the Reverse Pulfrich effect. The visual system may be able to adapt, or not, to the new optical condition, depending on the degree of the cataract and the magnitude of the monovision correction.
    METHODS: Here, we report a unique case of a 45-year-old patient that underwent unilateral cataract surgery resulting in a monovision correction of 2.5 diopters (D): left eye emmetropic after the surgery compensated with a monofocal intraocular lens and right eye myopic with a spherical equivalent of -2.50 D. This patient suffered severe symptoms in binocular vision, which can be explained by a spontaneous Pulfrich effect (a delay measured of 4.82 ms, that could be eliminated with a 0.19 optical density filter). After removing the monovision with clear lens extraction in the second eye, symptoms disappeared. We demonstrate that, at least in this patient, both Classic and Reverse Pulfrich effects coexist after unilateral cataract surgery and that can be readapted by reverting the interocular differences. Besides, we report that the adaptation/readaptation process to the Reverse Pulfrich effect happens in a timeframe of weeks, as opposed to the Classic Pulfrich effect, known to have timeframes of days. Additionally, we used the illusion measured in the laboratory to quantify the relevance of the spontaneous Pulfrich effect in different visual scenarios and tasks, using geometrical models and optic flow algorithms.
    CONCLUSIONS: Measuring the different versions of the Pulfrich effect might help to understand the visual discomfort reported by many patients after cataract surgery or with monovision and could guide compensation or intervention strategies.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    研究并比较由于晶状体引起的散光在近距离和远距离引起的近视性老年期视敏度(VA)的下降。
    招募了14名矫正近视老者。对于透镜引起的散光的不同条件,以双眼测量VA(最小分辨率角的对数):-0.25,-0.50,-0.75,-1.00,-1.50和-2.00屈光度的圆柱屈光力(和正球面屈光力的一半)将两个轴方向(规则WTR和反对规则ATR)添加到其光学校正中。在明视和中视条件下,在远距离和近距离进行测量,以及高和低对比度(HC/LC)刺激。使用配对Wilcoxon符号秩统计检验来评估条件之间的差异。
    在所有研究的实验条件下,通过回归线描述了测量的VA与晶状体诱导的散光的函数关系。这些线的角度系数(斜率)表示VA退化,ie,logMAR的变化对应于增加1.00屈光度的圆柱功率。在明视HC条件下,VA降解在远距离比近距离明显(在WTR条件下,0.22±0.06屈光度-1vs0.15±0.05屈光度-1,p=0.0061;在ATR条件下,0.18±0.06屈光度-1vs0.12±0.05屈光度-1,p=0.0017),尽管近处和远处零缸的VAs相似(-0.14±0.10vs-0.14±0.08,p=0.824)。
    在具有HC刺激的明视条件下,对晶状体诱导的散光模糊的近处比远距离更好的耐受性暂时归因于可能的经验介导的神经补偿,该神经补偿与眼睛倾向于近处的固有散光有关。
    UNASSIGNED: To investigate and compare the degradation of visual acuity (VA) in myopic presbyopes due to lens-induced astigmatism at near and at far distance.
    UNASSIGNED: Fourteen corrected myopic presbyopes were recruited. VA (logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution) was measured binocularly for different conditions of lens-induced astigmatism: cylindrical powers of -0.25, -0.50, -0.75, -1.00, -1.50, and -2.00 diopters (and positive spherical power of half the cylindrical power) with two axis orientations (with-the-rule WTR and against-the-rule ATR) were added to their optical correction. Measurements were carried out at far and near distance both in photopic and mesopic conditions, and for high and low contrast (HC/LC) stimuli. The paired Wilcoxon signed-rank statistics test was used to evaluate difference between conditions.
    UNASSIGNED: The measured VA as a function of the lens-induced astigmatism was described by regression lines in all investigated experimental conditions. The angular coefficients (slopes) of these lines represent the VA degradation, ie, the variation in logMAR corresponding to the addition of 1.00 diopters of cylindrical power. In photopic HC conditions, the VA degradation is significantly more pronounced at far distance than at near distance (0.22±0.06 diopters-1 vs 0.15±0.05 diopters-1, p = 0.0061 in WTR conditions; 0.18±0.06 diopters-1 vs 0.12±0.05 diopters-1, p = 0.0017 in ATR conditions), although VAs at near and at far with zero cylinder were similar (-0.14±0.10 vs -0.14±0.08, p = 0.824).
    UNASSIGNED: The better tolerance to lens-induced astigmatism blur at near than at far distance in photopic conditions with HC stimuli is tentatively attributed to a possible experience-mediated neural compensation associated to the tendency of the eye toward an inherent astigmatism at near.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    肠沙门氏菌中主要的生物膜途径涉及诱导csgA基因的特定生长条件,其产物形成介导生物膜形成的表面curli纤维。我们已经发现,鼠伤寒沙门氏菌中先前未表征的STM1266基因在通过curli途径调节生物膜形成中起作用。鼠伤寒沙门氏菌ΔSTM1266菌株显示生物膜缺陷,和STM1266的过表达导致生物膜形成增强。使用启动子-报告基因β-半乳糖苷酶测定,STM1266缺失导致csgA表达降低,和csgA和csgD缺失消除了STM1266过表达对生物膜形成的影响,而bcsA(编码纤维素形成的必需酶)的缺失没有影响。在小鼠感染模型中,ΔSTM1266菌株显示出与先前报道的ΔcsgA菌株相似的结果。预测STM1266基因编码MerR家族的DNA结合转录调节因子,并且与大肠杆菌BluR调节蛋白同源。我们恭敬地建议将名称brfS(鼠伤寒沙门氏菌的生物膜调节剂)归因于STM1266基因。
    The major biofilm pathway in Salmonella enterica serovar Typhimurium involves specific growth conditions that induce the csgA gene whose product forms surface curli fibers that mediate biofilm formation. We have found that the previously uncharacterized STM1266 gene in S. Typhimurium plays a role in regulating biofilm formation via the curli pathway. S. Typhimurium ΔSTM1266 strains display a biofilm defect, and overexpression of STM1266 results in enhanced biofilm formation. STM1266 deletion resulted in lowered csgA expression using promoter-reporter β-galactosidase assays, and csgA and csgD deletions abrogate the effects of STM1266 overexpression on biofilm formation while deletion of bcsA (encoding an essential enzyme for cellulose formation) has no effect. In a mouse infection model, the ΔSTM1266 strain displayed results similar to those seen for previously reported ΔcsgA strains. The STM1266 gene is predicted to encode a DNA-binding transcriptional regulator of the MerR family and is homologous to the Escherichia coli BluR regulator protein. We respectfully propose to ascribe the name brfS (biofilm regulator for Salmonella Typhimurium) to the STM1266 gene.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    据报道,各种体育任务中的专家和新手的行为不受模糊的引入。然而,研究使用了多种模糊视觉刺激的方法(即,屈光模糊和高斯模糊),和任务并不总是保持正常的感知-行动耦合。在目前的研究中,我们开发了一种新的实验协议,以使用沉浸式VR任务检查不同水平的高斯模糊对拦截性能和眼睛注视数据的影响。重要的是,这提供了真实世界拳击场景的真实模拟(例如,在不同的拳头组合开始之前存在一种假动作),其中专业的战斗运动员(n=18)经历了第一人称,视觉环境的适应性观点,根据他们的凝视位置(中央模糊,外围模糊,没有模糊)。我们发现,与没有模糊的对照条件相比,参与者在存在中央模糊或外围模糊的情况下表现出相似的拦截性能。然而,与周围模糊相比,中央模糊的拦截性能明显更好.眼睛凝视数据表明,尽管参与者专注于相似的感兴趣区域,而不管模糊的存在,与所有中央模糊水平和对照条件相比,在外围观察条件下,固定持续时间明显更长,模糊水平很高。这些发现可以通过将注意力转移到环境的不同区域来解释,从而影响了对显著信息的感知。与手臂假肢或无假肢相比,参与者在脚部假肢之前的序列的第一拳表现也更好。尽管如此,不管是假的类型,与第一冲头相比,第二冲头和第三冲头的性能明显更好。这些发现与参与者使用来自对手的身体运动和情境概率的额外信息来提高表现的顺序一致。总的来说,这些是使用VR作为检查注视环境操纵的手段的第一个证据,并因此突出了促进学习和转移到真实体育情境的潜力。
    It has been reported that behavior of experts and novices in various sporting tasks is impervious to the introduction of blur. However, studies have used diverse methods of blurring the visual stimulus (i.e., dioptric blur and Gaussian blur), and tasks that did not always preserve the normal perception-action coupling. In the current study, we developed a novel experimental protocol to examine the effect of different levels of Gaussian blur on interception performance and eye gaze data using an immersive VR task. Importantly, this provided a realistic simulation of a real-world boxing scenario (e.g., the presence of a feint prior to the onset of different combinations of punches) in which expert combat athletes (n = 18) experienced a first-person, adaptive viewpoint of the visual environment, which could be blurred according to their gaze location (central blur, peripheral blur, no blur). We found that participants exhibited similar interception performance in the presence of central blur or peripheral blur compared to a control condition with no blur. However, interception performance was significantly better with a central blur compared to peripheral blur. Eye gaze data indicated that although participants fixated at similar areas of interest irrespective of the presence of blur, fixation duration was significantly longer with a strong level of blur in the peripheral viewing condition than all levels of central blur and the control condition. These findings can be explained by relocating attention to different areas of the environment, which thereby influenced the perception of salient information. Participants also performed better on the first punch of a sequence preceded by a foot feint compared to arm feint or no feint. Still, irrespective of feint type, performance was significantly better on the second and third punch compared to the first punch. These findings are consistent with participants using additional information from the opponent\'s body movements and situational probabilities to increase performance as the sequence of punches developed. Overall, these are the first evidence for the use of VR as a means to examine gaze-contingent manipulations of the environment, and hence highlight the potential for facilitating learning and transfer to a real sporting situations.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目的:本文提出了一个简单的模型,该模型描述了在存在高阶像差(HOA)的情况下,视敏度与散焦(如在散焦曲线中)之间的定量关系。
    方法:描述视敏度与模糊之间关系的基础模型取自Blendowske作为起点,以开发适用于散焦曲线的扩展模型。Blendowskes数据的基础是通过合并HOA诱导的效应来扩展的。将屈光不正转换为单个模糊量b,也称为屈光距离,它在两个模型中都用作输入。不包括非对称像差的轴和瞳孔尺寸的可能影响。
    结果:扩展模型已用于为老花眼的双非球面多焦点矫正轮廓生成参考数据。它提供了与文献中针对该特定概况报道的类似的参考数据。
    结论:本文提出的模型可用于确定与HOA存在相关的焦深效应。扩展模型似乎为散焦曲线提供了有效的描述。无辅助视敏度实验数据的实际不确定性,尤其是经常缺乏有关瞳孔直径的信息,防止有意义的数值比较和两个模型的细化。然而,提供了支持扩展模型的理论论证。
    OBJECTIVE: This article proposes a simple model that describes the quantitative relationship between visual acuity and defocus (as in defocus curves) in the presence of higher-order aberrations (HOA).
    METHODS: The basis model for describing the relationship between visual acuity and blur was taken from Blendowske as a starting point to develop an extended model applicable to defocus curves. The basis of Blendowskes\'s data is extended by incorporating effects induced by HOA. Aberrometric refractive errors are transformed into a single blur quantity b, also termed dioptric distance, which serves as an input in both models. The possible influence of the axes of asymmetric aberrations and the pupil size is not included.
    RESULTS: The extended model has been used to generate reference data for a bi-aspheric multifocal correction profile for presbyopia. It provides reference data similar to what has been reported in the literature for that particular profile.
    CONCLUSIONS: The model proposed in this article can be used to determine depth-of-focus effects associated with the presence of HOA. The extended model seems to provide a valid description for defocus curves. Actual uncertainties in experimental data on unaided visual acuity, especially the frequent lack of information on pupil diameter, prevent meaningful numerical comparison and the refinement of both models. However, theoretical arguments are provided in support of the extended model.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    视觉模式可以唤起标记,即使是美丽的运动错觉,即使它们是静态的;眼球运动很可能是时间调制器。本文主要研究Ouchi型“相对”或“滑动”运动错觉。它概述了一个眼睛运动诱发的运动模糊假设,这不能正确预测最大错觉的偏移方向。这种失败导致了一种几乎新的特别简单的刺激:一种强烈唤起相对运动错觉的虚线排列,“正交的虚线摇摆。“后者很好地解释了运动积分。
    Visual patterns can evoke marked, even beautiful motion illusions even if they are static; eye movements in all likelihood serve as temporal modulators. This paper concentrates on Ouchi-type \"relative\" or \"sliding\" motion illusions. It outlines an eye-motion-evoked motion-blur hypothesis, which does not correctly predict the shift direction of maximal illusion. This failure led to a nearly new particularly simple stimulus: an arrangement of dashed lines that strongly evokes a relative motion illusion, the \"orthogonal dotted lines sway.\" The latter is well explained by motion integration.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

公众号