Timing

定时
  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:尽管有关于虐待性头部创伤的诊断和时机的研究,儿科专科医师之间的实践仍然存在差异。
    目的:使用婴儿头部损伤的案例,检查儿科专科医师的诊断变异性。次要目标是专家之间的时间可变性,以及虐待儿童儿科医生(CAPs)的诊断和时间变异性。
    方法:从儿童虐待儿科招募儿科专科医生,医院医学,急诊医学,重症监护,和神经外科来完成一个研究仪器。如果参与者在其职业生涯中评估了至少1例可能的虐待性头部创伤,则有资格参加该研究。
    方法:这种多机构,混合方法研究使用的研究仪器有4例婴儿头部损伤:严重的视网膜出血(RH),质量效应硬膜下出血(SDH),SDH膜形成,还有败血症.大多数CAPs选择的响应是参考,并使用卡方或Fisher精确检验在亚专业和CAPs之间进行比较。Bonferroni校正(p<0.01)用于亚专业比较。
    结果:有288名参与者完成了至少1个案例。在所有病例实例中均观察到诊断变异性。医院医学显著减少(34.9%vs.57.9%,p<0.01),急诊医学(28.0%vs.57.9%,p<0.0001),和神经外科(24.0%vs.57.9%,p<0.01)参与者选择了质量效应SDH案例示例的参考响应。所有病例的时间变异性均具有统计学意义(p<0.01)。对于重度RH病例示例,44-64岁的CAPs选择了时间参考响应(p<0.01)。
    结论:针对虐待性头部创伤中具有挑战性问题的其他同行评审过程和共识指南可能是有益的。
    Although there is research regarding the diagnosis and timing of abusive head trauma, there remains practice variation among pediatric subspecialists.
    To examine diagnostic variability among pediatric subspecialists using case examples of infant head injury. Secondary objectives were timing variability among subspecialists, and diagnostic and timing variability among Child Abuse Pediatricians (CAPs).
    Pediatric subspecialists were recruited from Child Abuse Pediatrics, Hospital Medicine, Emergency Medicine, Critical Care, and Neurosurgery to complete a research instrument. Participants qualified for the study if they evaluated at least 1 case of possible abusive head trauma during their career.
    This multi-institutional, mixed-methods study used a research instrument with 4 case examples of infant head injury: severe retinal hemorrhages (RH), mass-effect subdural hemorrhage (SDH), SDH membrane formation, and sepsis. The response selected by most CAPs was reference and compared across subspecialties and among CAPs using Chi-square or Fisher\'s exact tests. A Bonferroni correction (p < 0.01) was used for subspecialty comparisons.
    There were 288 participants who completed at least 1 case example. Diagnostic variability was observed in all case examples. Significantly fewer Hospital Medicine (34.9 % vs. 57.9 %, p < 0.01), Emergency Medicine (28.0 % vs. 57.9 %, p < 0.0001), and Neurosurgery (24.0 % vs. 57.9 %, p < 0.01) participants selected the reference response for the mass-effect SDH case example. Timing variability was statistically significant for all case examples (p < 0.01). Significantly fewer CAPs aged 44-64 years selected the reference response for timing (p < 0.01) for the severe RH case example.
    Additional peer review processes and consensus guidelines for challenging issues in abusive head trauma may be beneficial.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    BACKGROUND: Convalescent plasma therapy is used for the treatment of critically ill patients for newly discovered infectious diseases, such as coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pneumonia, under the premise of lacking specific treatment drugs and corresponding vaccines. But the best timing application of plasma therapy and whether it is effective by antiviral and antibiotic treatment remain unclear.
    METHODS: We describe a patient with COVID-19, a 100-year-old, high-risk, elderly male who had multiple underlying diseases such as stage 2 hypertension (very high-risk group) and infectious pneumonia accompanied by chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and emphysema. We mainly describe the diagnosis, clinical process, and treatment of the patient, including the processes of two plasma transfusion treatments.
    CONCLUSIONS: This provides a reference for choosing the best timing of convalescent plasma treatment and highlights the effectiveness of the clinical strategy of plasma treatment in the recovery period of patients with COVID-19 pneumonia.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    The continued spread of COVID-19 suggests a significant possibility of reimposing the lockdowns and stricter social distancing similar to the early phase of pandemic control. We present a dynamic model to quantify the impact of isolation for the contagion curves. The model is calibrated to the COVID-19 outbreak in Spain to study the effects of the isolation enforcement following the declaration of the state of alarm (14 March 2020). The simulations indicate that both the timing and the intensity of the isolation enforcement are crucial for the COVID-19 spread. For example, a 4-day earlier intervention for social distancing would have reduced the number of COVID-19 infected people by 67%. The model also informs us that the isolation enforcement does not delay the peak day of the epidemic but slows down its end. When relaxing social distancing, a reduction of the contagion probability (with the generalization of preventive actions, such as face mask wearing and hands sanitizing) is needed to overcome the effect of a rise in the number of interpersonal encounters. We report a threshold level for the contagion pace to avoid a second COVID-19 outbreak in Spain.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    背景:腹腔镜造口术在治疗腹内胃肠道灾难或创伤患者中很重要。它具有巨大的风险,并且是资源密集型的,无论是护理还是手术。主要目标是实现迅速的肌筋膜闭合(MFC),以最大程度地减少发病率和死亡率。早期MFC最初被定义为2-3周内,但越来越多的证据表明这应该以几天为单位进行测量。
    方法:回顾性分析了2016年至2018年在一个为50万人口服务的急性信任和创伤中心进行的腹腔镜造口术病例。指示,开放腹部(OA)的持续时间,检查了重新查看程序的数量和顾问的存在,以查看它们是否影响MFC费率,发病率和死亡率。
    结果:总体而言,在3年的研究期间,进行了76次腹腔镜造口术。最常见的适应症是腹膜炎(68.4%)。随着OA持续时间和复查程序数量的增加,MFC的机会大幅下降。在第1天之后,MFC速率在随后的每24小时下降20%。与术后并发症后进行腹腔镜造口术相比,在初次手术时将腹部开放与MFC发生率显着提高相关(92.6%vs68.2%,(p=0.006)。死亡率为15.8%。
    结论:如果OA在五天内或第三次复查程序未关闭,那么实现MFC是不可能的。应采用其他方法关闭腹部,而不是继续将患者带回手术室进行腹腔镜手术,同时增加发病率和死亡率的风险。在索引程序中形成原发性腹腔镜造口术的主动策略具有很高的闭合率。
    BACKGROUND: Laparostomy is important in the management of patients with intra-abdominal gastrointestinal catastrophe or trauma. It carries significant risk and is resource intensive, both in terms of nursing and surgically. The main goal is to achieve prompt myofascial closure (MFC) in order to minimise morbidity and mortality. Early MFC was initially defined as within 2-3 weeks but there is growing evidence that this should be measured in days.
    METHODS: Retrospective analysis was undertaken of laparostomy cases between 2016 and 2018 at an acute trust and trauma centre serving a population of 500,000. Indication, duration of open abdomen (OA), number of relook procedures and consultant presence were examined to see whether they affected MFC rates, morbidity and mortality.
    RESULTS: Overall, 76 laparostomies were performed during the 3-year study period. The most common indication was peritonitis (68.4%). As duration of OA and number of relook procedures increased, the chances of MFC fell significantly. After day 1, MFC rates fell by 20% with each subsequent 24 hours. Leaving the abdomen open primarily at index procedure compared with performing laparostomy following a postoperative complication was associated with significantly higher MFC rates (92.6% vs 68.2%, (p=0.006). The mortality rate was 15.8%.
    CONCLUSIONS: If the OA is not closed within five days or by the third relook procedure, then achieving MFC is unlikely. Alternative methods should be employed to close the abdomen rather than continuing to take the patient back to theatre for relook laparotomies while increasing the risk of morbidity and mortality. A proactive strategy to forming primary laparostomy at the index procedure has high closure rates.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Case Reports
    BACKGROUND: The treatment of diabetic foot ulcers in this case is complex and multidisciplinary, and an interdisciplinary team is extremely beneficial.
    METHODS: We performed the intervention on an old type 2 diabetes patient with poor health, whose left toes were severely necrotic. Surgery, including debridement and patella truncation, had positive effects on lower extremity circulation, infection control, cavity treatment, bone destruction, surgical debridement, recovery of foot function, and nursing. After 5 months, the patient\'s foot ulcer had healed, and walking function was preserved.
    CONCLUSIONS: Scheduling interventional surgery and debridement are the key point in a complicated diabetic foot ulcers case, and multidisciplinary collaboration in treatment of diabetic foot is significantly important.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

       PDF(Pubmed)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    目前,运动处方在很大程度上依赖于FITT原则中包含的参数:频率,强度,时间(持续时间),锻炼的类型。在本文中,包括时机的好处(FITT+T),指的是何时进行运动与进餐时间有关,正在讨论。目前的研究表明,时机是特定于结果的。总能量和脂质摄入量,餐前锻炼可以减少餐后高甘油三酯血症,而餐后运动改善了血糖控制。尽管研究结果表明,时间安排可以帮助肥胖管理和降低心脏代谢风险,大多数研究涉及成人受试者和急性调查。一些对孩子的研究,关于时机对食欲的影响,表明餐前锻炼有助于调节能量平衡,但也确定了与成年人相比在反应方面的关键差异。总的来说,目前的发现支持时机的好处,但是需要研究来建立针对儿科人群及其健康相关目标的指南,同时包含其他FITT组件。
    Currently, exercise prescription relies heavily on parameters included in the FITT principle: frequency, intensity, time (duration), and type of exercise. In this paper, the benefits of including timing (FITT+T), referring to when exercise is performed in relation to meal-time, is discussed. Current research indicates that timing is outcome-specific. Total energy and lipid intakes, and postprandial hypertriglyceridemia can be reduced when exercise is performed pre-meal, while glycemic control is improved with post-meal exercise. Although findings indicate that timing can aid in obesity management and cardiometabolic-risk reduction, most research involves adult subjects and acute investigations. Some research with children, concerning the effect of timing on appetite, indicates that pre-meal exercise helps regulate energy balance, but also identifies key differences in response compared with adults. Overall, current findings support the benefits of timing, but research is required to establish guidelines that are specific to the pediatric population and their health-related goals, while incorporating other FITT components.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    There is a known introspective limitation in the Psychological Refractory Period (PRP) paradigm - people underestimate the dual-task costs on their second reaction time. The prevailing explanation for this is that conscious awareness of the second stimulus is delayed in time until the first task has been centrally processed. Here, we examined this effect in more detail, by comparing reaction time estimates after processing a PRP task, and after passively experiencing \'replays\' of PRP trials. Even when participants had no dual-task processing demands, they did not accurately report the reaction time intervals using a visual analogue scale (the original reporting method of most introspective PRP experiments), but they did when placing markers that represent each event on a timeline. Thus, the timeline seems to better represent participants\' introspective representation of the trial. Importantly, introspection limitations still existed when participants processed the PRP task and then recreated it on a timeline.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    When young children answer questions, they do so more slowly than adults and appear to have difficulty finding the appropriate words. Because children leave gaps before they respond, it is possible that they could answer faster with gestures than with words. In this study, we compare gestural and verbal responses from one child between the ages of 1;4 and 3;5, to adult Where and Which questions, which can be answered with gestures and/or words. After extracting all adult Where and Which questions and child answers from longitudinal videotaped sessions, we examined the timing from the end of each question to the start of the response, and compared the timing for gestures and words. Child responses could take the form of a gesture or word(s); the latter could be words repeated from the adult question or new words retrieved by the child. Or responses could be complex: a gesture + word repeat, gesture + new word, or word repeat + new word. Gestures were the fastest overall, followed successively by word-repeats, then new-word responses. This ordering, with gestures ahead of words, suggests that the child knows what to answer but needs more time to retrieve any relevant words. In short, word retrieval and articulation appear to be bottlenecks in the timing of responses: both add to the planning required in answering a question.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

       PDF(Sci-hub)

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Temporal coordination between members of a string quartet was investigated across repeated performances of an excerpt of Haydn\'s string quartet in G Major, Op. 77 No. 1. Cross-correlations between interbeat intervals of performances at different lags showed a unidirectional dependence of Viola on Violin I, and of Violin I on Cello. Bidirectional dependence was observed for the relationships between Violin II and Cello and Violin II and Viola. Own-reported dependencies after the performances reflected these measured dependencies more closely than dependencies of players reported by the other players, which instead showed more typical leader-follower patterns in which Violin I leads. On the other hand, primary leadership from Violin I was observed in an analysis of the bow speed characteristics preceding the first tone onset. The anticipatory movement of Violin I set the tempo of the excerpt. Taken together the results show a more complex and differentiated pattern of dependencies than expected from a traditional role division of leadership suggesting several avenues for further research.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

  • 文章类型: Journal Article
    Reports of introspective reaction times (iRTs) have been used to investigate conscious awareness during dual-task situations. Previous studies showed that dual-task costs in RTs (the psychological refractory period, PRP, effect) are not reflected in participants\' introspective reports. This finding has been attributed to conscious awareness of Task 2 being delayed while Task 1 is centrally processed. Here, we test this Temporal model and compare it to an alternative that assumes participants base their iRTs on experienced difficulty. We collected iRTs and difficulty estimates after each trial of a PRP paradigm in which the perceptual difficulty of either Task 2 (Experiment 1) or Task 1 (Experiment 2) was manipulated. Our results largely support the difficulty-based account, suggesting that in a dual-task situation, iRTs do not reflect timing of cognitive processes but are strongly influenced by the experience of difficulty.
    导出

    更多引用

    收藏

    翻译标题摘要

    我要上传

    求助全文

公众号